Klinger University of Missouri Police Responses to Police Shootings 2001
Download original document:
Document text
Document text
This text is machine-read, and may contain errors. Check the original document to verify accuracy.
The author(s) shown below used Federal funds provided by the U.S. Department of Justice and prepared the following final report: Document Title: Police Responses to Officer-Involved Shootings Author(s): David Klinger Document No.: 192286 Date Received: February 01, 2002 Award Number: 97-IJ-CX-0029 This report has not been published by the U.S. Department of Justice. To provide better customer service, NCJRS has made this Federallyfunded grant final report available electronically in addition to traditional paper copies. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. OFFICER-INVOLVED SHOOTINGS* SHOOTINGS· POLICE RESPONSES TO OFFICER-INVOLVED PF;OPERTY OF National Criminal Justice Reference Service (NCJRS) Box 6000 RockvI!!e. f,fjO 20849-6000 • -"~.. " David Klinger Associate Professor of Criminology and Criminal Justice University University of Missouri-St. Missouri-St. Louis October 16,2001 16,2001 rn •Award -ex-0029 from the Office of Justice *Award number 97-IJ 97-IJ-CX-0029 Justice Programs, Programs, National Institute Institute of Justice, Justice, Department Department of Justice. Justice. Points Points of view in this document document are those of the author and do not necessarily necessarily represent represent the official position of the U.S. US.Department of Justice. Justice. This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • 0 necessarily of Justice. necessarily represent the official position of the U.S. Department of POLICE POLICE RESPONSES TO OFFICER-INVOLVED OFFICER-INVOLVED SHOOTINGS ABSTRACT ABSTRACT • a of literature Research Research on the use of deadly force force by police officers includes a limited body of that examines the consequences of involvement in shootings for officers who pull the trigger. of shootings: 1) what officers This This literature literature addresses two distinct issues related to the effects of after shooting incidents. Where the experience experience during shootings and 2) what they experiences ujler first issue is concerned, concerned, the research indicates that officers sometimes experience sensory issue is of time. Where postdistortions distortions such such as as tunnel vision, auditory blunting, and altered perceptions of of shooting shooting responses are concerned, the literature reports that officers may experience a variety of short of short and long-term reactions that can include recurrent thoughts about the incident a sense of numbness, of such responses numbness. trouble sleeping, sleeping, sadness, sadness, crying and nausea. nausea. Indeed, the existence of has has led mental mental health professionals who work with officers involved in shootings to identify them as "post-shooting as a type of post-traumatic stress response, commonly referred to as “post-shooting trauma." trauma.” research described in this report was undertaken to enhance understanding of The research of both of interviews with 80 aspects aspects of officers' officers’ reactions to involvement in shootings. shootings. It consisted of caseS where they shot citizens municipal municipal and county police officers who reported on 113 113 separate cases during their careers in law enforcement. The report offers a review of of what previous research has during reported about about officers' officers’ responses, describes the research procedures utilized in the current work, reported provides sketches sketches of the officers who participated in the current study and of of the incidents in provides shot other human beings, details what the research disclosed about officers’ which they shot officers' experiences during and after their shootings, shootings. and concludes with a discussion of of the academic experiences and policy ramifications of these findings. and • This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • POLICE RESPONSES TO OFFICER-INVOLVED SHOOTINGS Research on the use of deadly force of literature force by police officers includes a limited body of that examines examines the consequences of involvement in shootings for officers who pull the trigger. This of This literature literature typically conjoins what are actually two distinct issues related to the effects of shootings: I ) what officers officers experience during shootings and 2) what they experiences after shootings: I) incidents incidents in which they shoot. Where the first issue is concerned, the limited research indicates that many officers experience sensory distortions such as tunnel vision (perceiving but a small portion of what is of is present in the visual field), field), auditory blunting (the attenuation or exclusion of • a audible sounds), of the shooting are sounds), and altered perceptions oftime of time wherein some segment(s) of experienced in either slow or fast motion (see, e.g., Nielsen, 1981; Hom, 1986; 1986; 1981; Solomon and Horn, Campbell. 1992). 1992). Where post-shooting responses are concerned, the literature reports that Campbell. officers may experience a variety of short and long-term reactions. In the immediate aftermath of officers of shootings. officers may experience a variety of mental and/or emotional symptoms such as a numbness, anxiety, anxiety, and anger, as well as physical symptoms such as crying and nausea. sense of numbness, sense As time passes, passes, officers may lose their appetite, have trouble sleeping, experience recurrent As thoughts or "flashbacks" “flashbacks” of the shooting incident, feel guilty about injuring or killing another thoughts human. and/or and/or experience a host of other longer-term responses to the shooting event (see, e.g., human. 1 98 1 ; Solomon and Hom, Horn, ]986; 1986; Campbell, 1992). 1992). Indeed. the existence of Nielsen. 1981; of such has led mental health professionals who work with officers ink olved in shootings to responses has responses involved • 3 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • identify them as a type of post-traumatic stress response, commonly referred to as "post-shooting “post-shooting trauma" trauma” (e.g., (e.g., Hill, 1984; 1984; Nielsen, 1981; 1981; Strattonetal., Stratton et al., 1984). 1984). The research described in this report was undertaken to enhance understanding of both aspects of officers' officers‘ reactions to involvement in shootings. It consisted of interviews with 80 municipal and county police officers who reported on 113 113 separate cases where they shot citizens during their careers in law enforcement. enforcement. The balance of this report is devoted to delineating what the data collected during these interviews tell us about how shootings shootings affect police officers. It starts with a detailed review of what previous research has reported about officers' officers’ responses, describes the research procedures utilized in the current work, provides sketches of the officers who participated in the current study and of the incidents in which they shot other human beings, officers' experiences during and after their shootings, details what the research disclosed about officers’ • and concludes with a discussion of the research and policy ramifications of of these findings. THE LITERATURE ON RESPONSES TO SHOOTINGS Much of the literature on what officers experience during and after shootings consists of 1983; Burris, 1985; 1985; Shaw, expository essays based on non-systematic research (e.g., Bettinger, 1983; 1981). 1981). The few systematic studies that have been published provide a more detailed picture of Nielsen's (1 (1981) of 63 municipal, county, 98 1) study of how police shootings affect involved officers. Nielsen’s and state law enforcement officers who had shot suspects, for example. found that more than three-fourths of of them experienced some notable perceptual distortion during the event (e.g., tunnel vision, auditory blunting). Nielsen further reported that during the first week following the shooting more than 90% of of the study officers experienced at least one physical physical symptom, • such as nausea, headaches, and general fatigue, and that nearly 90% of of the shooters experienced 4 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I • 0 at at least one one emotional or psychological symptom, symptom, such as depression, anxiety, or intrusive thoughts thoughts about the incident. Finally, while Nielsen did not quiz officers about physical, emotional, emotional, and psychological responses during any time frame but the first week post-shooting, he he did ask them whether they experienced any attitude changes during the first three months following 80% of the officers Nielsen studied reported that they had; following their shootings. shootings. Nearly 80% increased apathy and cautiousness were the most frequently reported changes. 's (1984) Stratton Stratton et al. al.’s (1 984) examination of how involvement in shootings affected 60 Los Nielsen's earlier work. They Angeles deputy sheriffs sheriffs offered far less information than did Nielsen’s reported no data on reactions during shootings and provided only limited information on postshooting responses. responses. Among the highlights of their findings is that the average deputy "occasionally" "some" “occasionally” experienced recurring thoughts (flashbacks) about the shooting and had “some” • problems sleeping during the week immediately after the incident. They further reported reported a modest decrease decrease in the frequency of flashbacks flashbacks and sleep disturbances that deputies experienced as as time passed during the first three months after their shootings (Stratton et al. did not mention deputies‘ reactions after three months). They also reported that 63% of of whether they measured deputies' deputies surveyed either cried or experienced feelings of the deputies of depression, anger, and/or elation’ elation l at some (unspecified) (unspecified) point following the shooting. Finally, they reported that 30% of some of the deputies shooting affected them either “greatly” "a lot,” lot," that 34% of of them reported a indicated that the shooting "greatly" or “a “moderate” effect, and the remaining 36% reported that the shooting affected them either “a "moderate" "a little” or "not “not at all." all.” little" • 0 ’ I They used a single indicator that asked the officers’ officers' whether whether they experienced any of of these these four things things to to information. develop this information. 5 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e A more detailed picture of how shootings can affect involved officers comes from Horn's (1986) Solomon and Horn’s (1 986) questionnaire study of 86 Rocky Mountain-area officers who had shot suspects in the course of their duties. duties. They reported, for example, that during the events where they fired their weapons, 83% of the officers experienced some sort of time distortion, 67% experienced some sort of auditory distortion, and 56% experienced some sort of visual distortion. Where post-shooting experiences are concerned, Solomon and Hom Horn offered information about 18 18 specific emotional, emotional. psychological, and physical reactions that officers may have experienced. They reported, for example, that 58% of the officers felt a notable degree of anger in the wake of the shooting, 46% experienced substantial sleep difficulties, difficulties, and 44% had bothersome intrusive thoughts. They used information about the 18 18 symptoms to create a "trauma “trauma rating" rating” score for each study officer and asked officers to rate how well they had 0 • integrated the shooting into their life at the time the questionnaire was administered. administered. Solomon and Hom Horn then examined the relationships between these two measures of how the shootings shootings affected the officers and the degree of support the officers felt from various quarters (e.g., (e.g.. fellow officers) following the shooting. shooting. They reported that the more support officers felt, felt, the less severe their response. Finally, Solomon and Hom Horn reported that the information they used to develop the 3 7% of the officers surveyed experienced "mild" “mild” posttrauma rating scores indicated that 37% 28% had "severe'" “severe” -‘moderate” reactions, and the remaining 28% shooting reactions, 35% experienced "moderate" reactions. Unfortunately, the data they collected did not clearly delineate the time frame(s) frame(s) during which officers experienced the various reactions they reported, so it is not possible from officers’ reactions may have varied over time. Horn’s study to determine how officers' Solomon and Horn's • Horn examined officers' officers’ Two other studies that were published after Solomon and Hom 6 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • responses to shootings offered data on just one of the two major temporal components involved Gersons's (1 (1989) brief (i.e., reactions during shootings and post-shooting responses). The first, Gersons’s 989) brief study of37 of 37 Dutch officers who had been involved in shootings, reported only on post-shooting experiences. Among the more salient findings findings reported were that 76% of the officers “hyper-alertness,’‘ and experienced recurrent thoughts about the event, 68% reported a sense of "hyper-alertness," disturbances. Gersons did not offer data on the timing of officers' 43% of them had sleep disturbances. officers’ postshooting reactions, so it is not possible to derive information on how they might have changed over time. While Gersons's Gersons’s study was limited to officers' officers’ post-shooting reactions, Artwohl and Christensen (1997) (1 997) limited their work to data on responses during shootings, with a focus focus on perceptual distortions. The most notable thing about Artwohl and Christensen's Christensen’s work is that the • rates they report for many specific distortions are substantially higher than those reported in other research. research. While they report, for example, example. that 82% ofthe of the 72 U.S. officers they studied experienced tunnel vision, the highest rate previously reported is 44% (See Campbell, 1992, 1992. below). Similarly, where they report a heightened visual detail rate of 65%, the only other study 18%, and while the to report on this distortion (Solomon and Horn, 1986) reported a rate of 18%, previous high for rate of auditory blunting was 51% 5 1% (Solomon and Horn), Artwohl and sound. Christensen report that 88% of the officers in their research experienced a diminution of sound. The most comprehensive systematic systematic research to date on how involvement in shootings Campbell’s (1992) (1 992) study of special agents of the Federal Federal affects law enforcement officers is Campbell's (FBI). M. hich indicted indicted that FBI agents who are involved in shootings tend Bureau of Investigation (FBI), which • to have less severe reactions compared with their peers in state and local law enforcement. 7 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I/ II • 0 Campbell administered a 16-page questionnaire, which he augmented with structured face-toface 167 agents agents who had been involved in shootings in the years 1973-1989. He face interviews, interviews, to 167 presented his findings findings in three main categories: 1) physical and emotional responses at the time of the the shooting, shooting, 2) psychological responses following the shooting, and 3) physical and emotional responses after the shooting. Among the most frequent emotional and physical experiences experiences reported during shooting events were a sense of disbelief that the event was occurring (37%), (37%), a sense of increased physical strength (46%), (46%), tunnel vision (44%), and auditory blunting blunting (42%). (42%). The The instrument that Campbell used included items for agents to report on the emotional, psychological, and physical after effects they experienced during the first 24 hours, and then during during the rest of the first week following following their shootings. The instrument also asked agents to • @ report report any any changes changes that they may have experienced in their attitudes or emotional states during the first first six six months after the shootings. shootings. For whatever reason. Campbell limited his presentation the the data on agents' agents’ post-shooting reactions to what they reported experiencing during the first of the week following following the events. Highlights from this data include the information that at some week point(s) during during the first first week, 62% 62% of the agents experienced recurrent thoughts about the point(s) shooting, 29% 29% had dreams dreams about the shooting, 32% had problems sleeping, 24% were fatigued, shooting, and 25% had some sense of anxiety and/or tension. and Although Campbell presented no data on temporal variability in agents’ agents' responses, he did devote considerable considerable attention to the role that post-shooting events played in agents’ devote agents' adjustment. He wrote, wrote, for for example, example, that many agents felt that certain post-shooting events had a negative He • impact on them: 27% stated that they worried a great deal about the Bureau’s of impact Bureau's investigation of 8 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e their shooting, shooting, and 20% identified the news media as a major source of aggravation. On the flip side side of the coin, coin. 60% 60% of the agents agents believed that discussing their experiences with other agents who who had been involved in shootings shootings helped them to cope with their own shooting. Moreover, Campbell reported that agents who went through a systematic post-shooting mental health debriefing (the Program") typically experienced fewer negative (the Bureau's Bureau’s "Post-Critical “Post-Critical Incident Program”) consequences consequences in the wake of their shootings shootings than their peers who did not. While While the extant literature offers a substantial amount of information about what officers of how shootings affect experience experience during and after shootings, shootings, we still have a very limited picture of police officers. We know, for example, that perceptual distortions are a common occurrence during during shootings, but we have virtually no information on the inter relationships between different types of distortions, distortions, how perceptions might change during shooting events, or how • Similarly. while we know that some distortions distortions might be associated with othetphenomena. other phenomena. Similarly, officers ot‘fcers experience experience specific physical, emotional, emotional, and psychological reactions to involvement in a shooting, shooting, previous research has not clearly specified how officers' officers‘ responses vary over time and we have little little understanding of how post-shooting reactions might be associated with other we factors. The The research described below was undertaken to provide more information about issues factors. as these. these. such as RESEARCH PROCEDURES Data were gathered via face-to-face interviews with a sample of of municipal and county officers and sheriffs sherifrs deputies deputies who had shot citizens (see (see below for the sampling police officers employed). The interviews interviews included two parts. of the procedures employed). parts. The first consisted of • administration of a substantially modified and expanded kersion version of of the interview schedule that administration 9 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. m• John John Campbell Campbell (1992) (1 992) used in his study of FBI agents. agents. The instrument used in the current study included included versions versions of nearly all of the items Campbell used in his instrument, with modifications to to either a) a) render the schedule schedule relevant to local and county officers (e.g., items that referred to "the “the bureau" bureau” were were altered) or b) to increase clarity. The current instrument also included numerous items not found found in Campbell's, Campbell’s, which were added to obtain more comprehensive measurement of specific topics (e.g., the weapon[s] possessed by subjects, a more complete inventory of what officers experienced during shooting incidents, and temporal variability in officers' of which is presented in officers’ reactions reactions following shootings). In total, the instrument (a copy of the of 144 major sets of items that covered the following broad areas of the Appendix) included 144 interest: • • e Background information about the officer, such as demographic characteristics, law enforcement experience, experience, and assignment at time of shooting. • Features of the shooting event, such as the number of suspects involved, their weapons, took, the actions that the subject officer and any other officers present the actions they took. took, and the nature of injuries incurred by officers, suspects, and other citizens. took, • The thoughts, feelings, feelings, and perceptions that subject officers experienced during the The shooting shooting incident. • Their physical. physical, psychological, and emotional experiences after the shooting. • The treatment that the subject officers received from others (e.g., family members, fellow officers, their agency) agency) following the shooting. officers, After completing a separate questionnaire for each incident in which they shot citizens • (see details below), each officer met with the Principal Investigator (PI) for an individual audio(see 10 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • taped, taped, directed interview that was later transcribed. transcribed. The interviews, which lasted between fortyin, how five minutes and three hours (depending on how many shootings the officers had been in. many of the various sorts sorts of possible responses they experienced, and how expressive they were with their answers), answers), focused on five five major areas that were addressed in the following order: 1) officers' officers’ backgrounds and their experiences, thoughts, and feelings regarding deadly force prior to of deadly force to their law enforcement careers, 2) the training they received regarding the use of during the academy and the field field training portion of their careers, 3) situations where they did not fire fire during circumstances where they believe that the use of deadly force would have been legally justified, 4) what occurred during the hours that immediately preceded the shootings. shootings, the circumstances circumstances of the shootings, shootings, and what transpired during the shootings, and 5) what occurred • a in in the aftermath of the shootings. shootings. The of the data The five five phase stmcture structure was designed to enhance the accuracy and detail of during the directed interview. The first two stages allowed the subject officers to ease collected during into the topic of deadly force. force. and allowed the PI to build a rapport with them while discussing discllssing into and socialization into police work. The third stage served as a transition during their background and focus of the interview shifted from more general and abstract issues regarding policing which the focus and deadly force force to the more specific issue of particular confrontations with citizens. Starting the and fourth phase with questions about what occurred in the hours preceding the shootings focused fourth officers’ minds on these critical events in an indirect fashion that was intended to get the officers officers' thinking in a sequential tashion, and thereby to facilitate recall about what transpired during situations in which they shot citizens. Finally, the fifth phase of the interview moved situations • frames addressed in the instrument (i.e., what transpired sequentially through the four time frames 11 11 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • a during the first 24 hours, the first week, the first three months, and after three months) in order to allow officers to continue with the sequential presentation their narratives. Officers' Officers’ responses to questionnaire questionnaire items guided the last two phases of the directed interviews (i.e., the shootings and their aftermath). aftermath). This served two purposes. First, First. by giving officers a chance to describe in their own words their thoughts, feelings, feelings, and experiences, the directed interviews yielded details about attitudes, emotions, experiences, and events that could not be obtained from a questionnaire, questionnaire, thus creating a more detailed picture of officers' officers’ involvement with and reactions to the use deadly force. Second, because the directed interviews covered much of the ground addressed in the questionnaire, they provided a reliability check on officers' officers’ responses to questionnaire items. This was accomplished by informally recounting back to the officers what they had reported on the instrument (e.g., "So “So prior to firing your weapon weapon you • started to experience things in slow motion, but no other sorts sorts of distortions"). distortions”). This process occasionally identified mistakes that officers had made when they marked the questionnaire (e.g., (e.g., frame something that failing to report something they experienced, or reporting in one time frame actually occurred in a different one) and sometimes yielded additional information about some facet of officers' officers’ experiences that they had not marked (e.g., "Now “Now that I think about it. my sense of visual detail was increased because I could clearly see the hairs on the suspect's suspect’s ann arm standing up"). up”). When an interview yielded additional information about some issue, or disclosed that the officer had responded incorrectly to an instrument item, the PI told the officer that he M \\ ished to alter the instrument to reflect the correct response to the relevant item and then, after ohtaining the officer's officer’s permission to make the correction, did so in his or her presence. • nou turns to a With a discussion of the data collection methodology in place, attention now ]2 12 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e discussion of how the officers who were interviewed came to be included in the present study. Selecting the Sample As the literature review indicates. police shootings can be extremely stressful events. Even when involvement in a shooting does not produce notable disruption in officers' officers’ lives, moreover, officers are often quite wary about discussing their experiences during and after situations in which they shot people with individuals outside their circle of close acquaintances. acquaintances. This is particularly so when a stranger seeks them out with a request to discuss their experiences in great detail. Because many police officers who have been in shootings would be disinclined to discuss the incidents and their aftermath in a frank manner with researchers who are complete strangers (even when the researcher brings the pledge of legally guaranteed confidentiality; see below), the research employed a sampling strategy intended to reduce the social distance between • the PI and the research subjects. The first step in the sampling process actually occurred before the proposal for the present research was submitted to the National Institute of Justice (NIl). (NIJ). Prior to submitting the NIl funding funding and the proposal, the PI secured agreements to participate in the study (contingent on NIJ confidentiality provisions that would obtain under 42 United States Code 3789g) from several shootings during their careers in law enforcement. The acquaintances who had been involved in shootings step, the PI informed other acquaintances funding was secured. In this step, second step occurred after funding in the police community about the project and asked them for help identifying officers (including sheriffs deputies) who might be willing to be interviewed. inter1,iewed. After this, the PI asked the pool of subject officers developed via the first two steps to help identify others who might be willing to • participate in the study. study. 13 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I • e This "snowball" “snowball” methodology resulted in interviews with 81 8 1 officers from 19 19 municipal and county law enforcement agencies in four states. One officer’s interview was excluded from the sample because none of the rounds he fired in the sole shooting in which he was involved struck anyone. Two other officers interviewed were involved in a major shoot-out during which they and several other officers exchanged numerous shots with two citizens who were barricaded inside a house that eventually burned to the ground. The post-shooting investigation disclosed that both citizens had suffered gunshot wounds before their bodies were burned beyond recognition. While the damage done by the fire precluded a conclusive determination about the source of the bullet wounds, the officers’ positions and actions during the shoot-out suggest that each of them did strike at least one of the citizens with gunfire. Consequently, these officers’ interviews were retained in the sample. Each of the 78 other officers who participated in the @ study were involved in at least one shooting in which bullets they fired definitely struck at least one citizen. There are two reasons why strategic infom1ant informant sampling was employed in this research. The first first was to reduce bias in the form of non-response non-rehponse that would almost certainly have obtained had a traditional probability sampling strategy been employed. The second was to enhance the internal validity of research conclusions. Probability sampling techniques are because they are designed to produce research generally preferred over other sampling strategies hecause findings with a high degree of generalizability. But they are not the best for all research settings, and there is reason to suspect that probability sampling samlding would have actually produced findings with less validity than those produced by the research described in this report • selection of all officers who had shot in a Any sort of probability sampling (a random sdection 14 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e randomly selected sample of police agencies) would have required that I work with the knowledge and blessings of police administrators. Because the research would have been linked with the agency. officers with negative feelings feelings toward their agency (a fairly common occurrence in the police world even absent involvement in shootings) or who did not trust their agency (another common sentiment among police officers) would be negatively disposed toward participation. Similarly, because the I would have been an unknown entity among the rank and file officers of the agencies that would have been selected, probability sampling would bias the sample towards officers who are trusting of outsiders (an (an uncommon police virtue). Indeed, several of the officers I interviewed told me the only reason that they agreed to participate in the study was that they either knew me personally or knew the informant who vouched for my qualifications. (I did not ask the officers why they agreed to participate. Several integrity and qualifications. • simply offered the above information.) Thus, while one can not know the sorts of bias introduced in the present sample by the use of the strategic informant technique, it did -- at a minimum -- substantially reduce non-response bias and thereby enhance the external validity of the study. Where internal validity is concerned, it is doubtful that those officers who eventually did participate in a study based on probability sampling would be forthcoming about questionable behavior in which they may have engaged, any negative assessments they might have about how they were treated in the wake of their shootings, and problems they might have experienced. As detailed later in the report, several of the officers I interviewed told me that they lied to the mental health professionals (MHP) to whom they were sent by their agency about how they were .. feeling in the wake of their shootings because they believed that what they told the MHPs might 15 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • a get back to their superiors. Given this, it is logical to suspect that some number of officers selected through a probability sampling design would be less than completely truthful with a department-approved researcher who wandered into their lives asking extremely intimate, and potentially incriminating, questions. As the snowball methodology produced a sample of officers who willingly participated in the research because they had either a personal or once removed link with the researcher, it is almost certain that the sampling technique used in the present study increased the internal validity of the findings findings produced. The 80 officers in the final sample were involved in 147 147 incidents where they discharged their firearms. firearms. Forty-five of the officers were involved in one incident, incident, 21 2 1 were involved in two, 5 were involved in three, 6 were involved in four, four, 3 were involved in five, and 1 was involved in six shootings. Counted among these 147 147 incidents were those in which the officers fired at a • citizens and missed. shot inanimate objects (such as motor vehicles), shot animals, accidently fired their weapons, weapons. and other sorts of cases where no humans were struck stnic k by study officers' officers’ gunshots. Because the study was undertaken to examine officers' officers’ responses to shooting humans, the officers did not complete questionnaires regarding incidents in which their bullets struck no one. one. Also, two officers who had been involved in multiple shootings could not spare the time to shootings, the report on all of them. One of them completed questionnaires for four of his six shootings, other for three of his four. four.*2 Both were briefly queried about the other shootings during the directed interviews. interviews. questionnaire, 16 completed two, 7 In the end. 56 of the study officers completed a single questionnaire, four. The interview process thus resulted in 113 1 13 completed three. and I1 officer completed four. • two officers ’ The tivo ofticers selected the shootings they complete questionnaires on with no direction Girection from the PI. 2 16 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. II • a 3 completed completed questionnaires questionnaires regarding regarding 113 1 13 instances instances in which study study officers officers shot citizens. citi~ens.~ FINDINGS I Presentation of what analysis analysis of the 113 1 13 shooting incidents incidents disclosed begins with background information about the officers officers and the circumstances of their shootings. shootings. In order to to facilitate facilitate speedy comprehension of this material, material, it is is presented in outline form. form. OFFICERS PROFILE OF STUDY OFFICERS follows provides some sense sense of these subject officers officers and The bulleted information that follows shootings they reported on in this study. the shootings • female officers. The sample includes 74 male officers and 6 female • AsiadPacific Sixty-two of the officers were white, nine were Hispanic, four were Asian/Pacific 0 • 0 “other” Islander, three were black, and two described themselves as having some "other" (e.g., Native American). racial/ethnic background (e.g., •0 21 to 49, with a mean The ages of these officers at the time of the shootings ranged from 21 of 32. • The amount of time they had spent as police officers prior to the shootings ranged from less than a year to 27 years, with a mean of just under 8 years. ofjust • 0 The vast majority of the research subjects (75) held the rank of "police “police officer” officer" (or “deputy "deputy sheriff’) sheriff') at the time of of the shootings; four were sergeants; and one was involved 3 • 3 Because the study was undertaken to examine individuals officers’ officers' responses to events in ill which they shot people, each officer‘s experiences as they pertain to a given shooting are treated as separate cast‘s. officer's cases. The vast majority of 9 8 ) were completely independent of of the cases in the sample ((98) of one another, bur but I5 15 of of them resultcd resulted from incidents where other officers in the sample also shot the citizen. These non independent cases involved iia total of of seven shootings; six in which two of the officers intervicwed intervit'wed shot the suspect(s) and one in which tlirrte three did. 17 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. ( • a in shootings both prior to and after being promoted to sergeant. sergeant. CIRCUMSTANCES CIRCUMSTANCES OF SHOOTINGS SHOOTINGS •e Nearly half (54) ofthe of the shootings occurred while the officers involved were working general patrol assignments. assignments. Because the sample included a disproportionate number of officers whose work includes assignment to their agency's special weapons and tactics (SWAT) teams,4 teams," a substantial minority of the shootings (37) (37) occurred during tactical operations. Fourteen of the SWAT shootings involved barricaded subjects, 6 were whi Ie officers served high-risk search and arrest warrants hostage incidents, 14 14 occurred while (14), (14), and the remaining 3 took place during miscellaneous SWAT SWAT activities. The 22 other shootings occurred during an array of circumstances that include undercover work, • crime suppression patrol, and off-duty situations. e * • Other officers were present in 103 103 of the shootings and fired shots in 51 5 1 of them. •e Subject officers faced a single suspect in more than three-fourths (89) of the shootings, two suspects in 13 13 shootings, shootings. three suspects in 5 others, four suspects in 4 instances, and five each. five and six suspects in 1 shooting each. •e In the 89 shootings in which officers faced a single opponent, the suspect was armed with some type of firearm 21 cases, firearm in 56 cases, knives or other edged weapons (e.g., axes) in 21 (such as baseball bats, toy guns, and vehicles) in 9 other miscellaneous other weapons (such cases, and no weapons (i.e.. the suspect was unarmed) in 3 cases. Among the 56 suspects J 4 • a For the last few years the PI has been involved imolved in research regarding and training of special weapons and (SWAT) Consequently. many of his contacts in the law enforcement community have tactics (S WAT) teams and officers. Consequently. been or are involved in SWAT work. As a result. many of the officers initially sought to participate in this project have SWAT experience. Given the nature of social networks, many of the officers referred to the PI by the officers in the initial pool also have SWAT backgrounds. The potential ramifications of the over-sample of SWAT shootings is addressed later in in this report. 18 18 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • who who were were armed with guns, guns, 30 had a single handgun, 11 11 possessed a single shotgun, 5 firearms~ and 2 others carried carried carried a single single rifle, 8 others were stocked with multiple firearms, both a firearm firearm and some other type of weapon. weapon. •a In In one one of the 24 cases with multiple multiple suspects, suspects, none of the suspects carried any weapons. In 12 of 12 of the remaining 23 23 cases, only one suspect was armed, most often with a firearm of some (N=lO). Among the 11 1 1 cases where multiple suspects possessed weapons, at some sort (N=10). of opponents least least two of the suspects suspects carried carried firearms firearms in 9 of them. The largest number of any any officer faced was six, six, five five of whom were armed with guns. In this case, the other four four suspects suspects dropped their weapons and surrendered immediately after the officer shot the first first gunman. gunman. The most extreme extreme multiple-suspect case involved an officer who was of whom were by himself when he engaged in a gun battle with four bank robbers, three of • armed armed with assault rifles. rifles. The officer somehow managed to incapacitate one robber and drive drive the rest away while sustaining sustaining only minor injuries himself, even though the only gun he carried jammed early in the firefight. tirefight. •a 113 cases, 60 suspects died. died, 43 incurred wounds that required hospitalization, Across the 113 while 5 others received minor wounds.’ wounds. 5 One of of the suspects who received minor wounds definitely would have suffered far more serious wounds (perhaps fatal ones) if definitely if not for the body armor he was wearing, which prevented the subject officers’ officers' rounds from case. a suspect who suffered severe gunshot wounds to penetrating his torso. In another cast.. if his body armor had not stopped his head and legs almost certainly Lvould \vould have died if • 0 The number of of suspects shot sums to les4 ,’ The less than the number of of cases because the sample includes seven incidents where more than one of the involvcd shooting incidents involved officers was interviewed interviewed. See footnote 33 above for for additional information. inhrination. additional 19 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. ( • e other rounds fired by the subject officer from entering his chest cavity. cavity. •e Subject officers received injuries requiring hospitalization in six cases and minor injuries in eight others. Among the officers whose wounds required hospital treatment were five five struck by gunfire and one who suffered a severe laceration when his assailant slashed him with a butcher knife. The most serious injuries suffered by a subject officer were caused by a through-and-through gunshot that traversed the officer's torso from front to back. The extensive internal damage caused by the bullet included such massive bleeding that officer's heart stopped beating on three separate occasions before medical personnel the officer's could stabilize her. • • 0 13 cases, 1 of them fatally. Other officers were injured in 13 • Citizens suffered non-fatal injuries at the hands of suspects in eight cases and fatal injuries in two others. With this information about the officers and the circumstances of their shootings in hand, shootings. The task begins with a attention now turns to officers' responses to involvement in shootings. discussion that identifies the specific information the current study sought about what officers experience during shootings. RESPONSES DURING SHOOTINGS sorts of ot' experiences officers The current study includes information about two distinct sorts 1) thoughts and feelings and 2) perceptual distortions. may have had during shooting events: I) Previous studies of such responses have treated shootings as unitary events. simply measuring and reporting whether officers experienced specific phenomena during the situation where they • fired. fired. 20 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. i ( • e oftraumatic Research on human responses during other sorts of traumatic events, however, indicates that individuals' experiences can vary over the course of a given stressful episode (e.g., Girelli et al., aI., individuals’ 1986). 1986). Consequently, the instrument used in the current study queried officers about thoughts/ feelings and perceptual distortions during two distinct points in the shooting incidents: 1) prior to firing weapons and 2) the moments during which and immediately after they fired their guns. Where thoughts/feelings are concerned, the instrument included items for officers to indicate whether they experienced • •a a sense of disbelief •a fear for self •a fear for others •a a need to survive m a • a rush of strength or adrenalin •a intrusive thoughts about irrelevant matters •a any other specific thoughts or feelings. feelings. Regarding perceptual distortions, distortions, the instrument used in the current research included items that queried officers about whether they experienced any of the following phenomena prior to firing firing and upon/after upordafter firing: firing: • •a tunnel vision vision tunnel •a heightened visual visual acuity acuity heightened •a diminished sound sound diminished •a amplified sound sound amplified •a time passing more slowing than usual (i.e, (i.e, slow motion) motion) 21 21 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • a • time passing more quickly than usual (i.e., fast motion) • any other perceptual distortions. of officers' officers' responses The data set thus includes information about four distinct categories of during shootings: perceptual shootings: 1) 1) thoughts/feelings prior to discharging their weapons, 2) perceptual distortions prior to discharging their weapons, 3) thoughtdfeelings thoughts/feelings w upon o n and after discharging their weapons, 4) perceptual distortions upon and after discharging their weapons. For simplicity's sake, "while" sake, the second time frame will henceforth be referred to with terms such as "while" and "as" "as" firing. firing. The presentation of what the current research disclosed about officer' responses of the two time points, then move on during shootings will first address thoughts/feelings at each of to to perceptual distortions. • a Thoughts/Feelings Though ts/Feel ings In order to develop some sense of how the current data compare with what previous inquiries have reported about officers' mental and emotional experiences during shootings, the first of the several sorts first analytical step step taken was to find out how often officers experienced each of feelings at ut any uny time during the 113 113 shootings studied. Overall, officers reportcd of thoughts and feelings reported 10 (97%) experiencing at least one of the thoughts/feelings thoughtdfeelings listed in the instrument in 1110 (97%) of of the experiencing shootings. In the specific specific response categories, officers experienced fear for others in 60%, a ruch shootings. rush of strength or adrenalin rush in 55%. disbelief in 42% of the cases. fear for self in 41 %, need tl' 1 survive in 30%, intrusive thoughts in 14%, 14%, and miscellaneous '-other" \''6 survive "other" emotions/thoughts in 33·"'0 ith what previous of the shootings. Table 1 compares these figures (where possible) M with • 22 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. a • research has reported. reported.66 The table shows that officers in the current study more frequently frequently experienced each of the specific specific responses responses that Campbell (1992) (1992) measured in his FBI subjects. subjects. It also shows that the officers in the current study less frequently experienced fear for self and more frequently experienced fear for others than did the officers that Neilsen (1980) (1 980) surveyed, surveyed, and that they less frequently had intrusive thoughts than did the officers in Artwhol and Christensen's Christensen’s (1997) (1997) research. With this information information in hand, attention now turns to the more fine-grained fine-grained look at what officers think and feel feel during shootings, shootings, which is provided by the current study's study’s measurement of these responses during two distinct segments oftime of time (i.e., prior to and while pulling the trigger). • INSERT TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE ofthe thoughts! Prior to pulling the trigger, officers experienced at least one of the six specific thoughts/ feelings 102 (90%) of of the cases, while in 6 others the involved feelings delineated on the instrument in 102 "other" specific thought or feeling. feeling. Thus, sutject subject officers reported experiencing only some “other” least one specific thought or feeling prior to shooting in 96% of the officers recalled having at feast of each thought/feeling prior to firing, firing, officers reported cases studied. Regarding the prevalence of safety of others in 54%. a rush of of strength or adrenalin in 44%. fear for experiencing fear for the safetc that the incident was happening in slightlj their own safety in 35%, of disbelief disbeliefthat slightly less than 35%, a sense of • 0 6 In this, and all other tables that compare data from the current research with that reported in previous studies how shootings affect police officers, studies that did not provide any information about the specitic subject in question are simply excluded froin from the table. In Table 1, lor for example, Solomon and Horn Hom (1986), Strattoil Stratton et al. ( 1984), 1984), and Gersons (1989) are all absent because none provided provided any data on officers’ officers' thoughts/feelings thoughts/feelings during shootings. J'l _J This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I/ Ii a • one-third (32%) of the shootings, a need to survive in just over a quarter (27%), (27%), intrusive thoughts about irrelevant matters in 10%.7 1O%.7 and some "other" “other” thought or feeling in 29% 29% of the cases. cases. Counted among these "other" “other” thoughts/feelings were concerns concerns about the tactical situation that the officers faced (e.g., being in a cross-fire with other officers); apprehension apprehension about the placement of shots officers were about to fire; fire; a sense sense of calm; anger at the suspect for trying to harm them or some innocent third party; attention to the weapon the suspect carried; and, in one case, case, a reaction that the officer could only describe as thinking "Oh, “Oh, Shit!!" Slit!!” just before being run down by a suspect driving a truck. As was the case prior to firing their weapons, officers reported at least one specific thought or feeling as or immediately after they fired in 96% of the cases. They experienced fear disbelief in 34% of the cases, cases. for others in 49%, a sense of strength or adrenalin rush in 46%, disbelief • fear for self in 30%, a need to survive in 23%, intrusive thoughts in 9%, and some other specific thought or feeling in 30% of the cases. Officers experiencing such "other" “other” responses reported. among other things, being angry at the suspect, wondering if the shots they were firing were justified, concern that the suspect did not go down immediately upon being struck with the initial bullets fired, and a sense of confusion confusion over oker what was happening because they did not realize that they had just fired (see discussion on pages 44 and 45 belou below for more on this point). The figures regarding officers' officers’ thoughts thoughts and feelings a both time period5 periods are presented in Table 2 below. INSERT TABLE 2 ABOUT HERE INSERT • focus on officers' officers’ loved ones. This!s This 1s also the case where intrusive thoiichts 7 These thoughts tended to ti)Cus thoughts iipoti firing tiring goes. goes. upon 24 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • The The Role ofFear of Fear One One thing that stands out in the foregoing foregoing discussion is that it appears that many officers experienced no sense sense of fear fear either prior to or as they shot, which at first might strike one as odd inasmuch as as the standard for the justifiable use of deadly force in law enforcement is that officers perceive that their life or limb, limb, or the life or limb of a third party, is in imminent peril. A more detailed look look at the data indicates that the frequencies frequencies reported above do not tell the entire story, of fear reported (i.e., fear for self self however. however. First, moving from from the gross rates of the four aspects of prior to to firing, firing, fear fear for others prior to firing, firing, etc.) into case-specific frequencies indicates that 70% 70% of the the time (N=79) officers felt fear either for themselves, others, or both, prior to firing, as of the cases did study officers they fired, fired, or at both time periods. Thus, in a substantial majority of 0 • experience experience a sense sense of fear for someone's safety at some point during the shootings in which they Second, information developed during the directed interviews indicates that the were involved. Second, officers who who did not report feeling fearful often recalled that they believed that their safety. officers safety, the safety of a third party, or both, was in jeopardy at some point in their shootings. Many of the officers who had not indicated on the questionnaire that they felt fearful of the indicated during the directed portion of the interviews that they perceived that the actions of suspect(s) they shot had placed their safety, the safety of another, or both in imminent peril, but suspect(s) fear. Thus, Thus. the negative responses to the "fear" they had not experienced the emotion of fear. "fear" that they items on the questionnaire were indicative not of the fact that some officers did not believe that items anyone's life was in danger, but rather simply that the intellectual understanding that they oorr • 25 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • someone trepidation. s someone else else was in extreme danger did not translate into emotional trepidation.’ of the fear that The directed interviews also also provided additional insights into the nature of officers officers experienced. experienced. Perhaps Perhaps the the most interesting impression in this connection is that what , officers is not always commensurate with the actual degree of officers experience experience in terms of fear fear is immediate immediate threat posed to self and others by suspects. Two officers who shot unarmed suspects, for for example, example, were quite fearful fearful that they were about to be shot themselves because they believed that that the the suspects suspects in fact fact possessed guns. guns. At the other extreme, an officer who was seriously injured injured by a gunman who shot her as she was drawing her weapon did not experience any fear for her safety safety either before or after she began to return fire. fire. of officers’ officers' Other cases illustrate a different twist on the objective threat theme: the object of fear fear is is not always always the person in the most immediate danger. In one such case, an officer who shot • of two two armed armed suspects suspects at the end of a vehicle pursuit that terminated in the empty front yard of private private home reported that while he felt no fear for himself. he was quite fearful (both before and upon firing) of them were present when the firing) for the residents of the house, even though none of ’ • 0 S The case of an officer who shot a suspect immediately after being slashed with an large knife (opening (opening a wound that required more than five dozen stitches to close) is illustrative of of how officers who shoot can have an intellectual appreciation of danger, but experience no fear. He reported that he was thinking: “I don't don’t want to get cut again. 1I could start feeling [my] .... I1 "I [my) clothes get wet. I1 knew I1 was bleeding and and.... don’t want to get cut again. The only way 1I can solve this real fast is to shoot this guy and take him down.” don't down." He went on to report that: “never at any point along the way was [there] "never [there] any feeling like fear as a cold feeling in the pit pit of ofyour your ‘Oh my God, I’m stomach, 'Oh I'm going to die’ die' or ‘I’m 'I'm really hurt.’ hurt.' That never never at any point from start to to finish finish there have been times [on this Job] [crossed my mind]. Oh, there job] when I’ve I've been scared shitless, shitless, ready to piss my Absolutely. But this, it just never, never happened here. Fear means an actual physical pants. Absolutely. physical reaction reaction that feel where you YOLIare in fear where you feel absolutely you feel absolutely life threatened. threatened. This was really really honestly more that.“ detached than that.'· The officer also reported that he was surprised at how calm he felt during and after the shooting, describing his mindmindset as one of being in a "problem-solving" “problem-solving” mode, relating that he -‘has "was really surprised” surprised" that he had had no no fear of of dying, dying, reporting and that he was "shocked" “shocked” at that because he “thought "thought that there would be [fear]” [fear]" under those circumstances. 26 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e shooting if he did not shoot the gunmen shooting occurred. The The officer explained that he was worried that if pointed 41; piss weapon toward toward the (one (one of whom was running toward the house when he turned and pointed pursuing pursuing officer) that they would invade the house and take the family hostage. In another case involving involving fear for for others, the officer involved in the wild shootout with four bank robbers mentioned on page 19 19 reported that his feelings of fear shifted during the incident. Prior to firing his his first shot, he was fearful fearful for both himself and other people in the bank, believing that everyone present was in grave danger of being killed. Once he started shooting, however, the fear fear he felt felt for for himself evaporated as the sense that he had to protect the bank customers and of the employees employees took over. Thus, even though the officer was in a furious gun battle where all of shots shots fired by the robbers were directed at him, the only fear he felt was for others. • 0 Perceptual PerceDtual Distortions of As was the case with thoughts/feelings, thoughts/feelings, the first analytical step taken in the analysis of was to find out how often officers experienced perceptual distortions at any altered perceptions was 1 13 shootings studied. Overall, officers reported experiencing at least one time during the 113 distortion (including "other") --other”)in 107 107 (95%) of the cases. The single distortion most commonly sound, which occurred in 82% of of the cases. On the flip side of of the experienced was diminished sound, coin, officers perceived some noises as being exceptionally loud in 20% of of the cases. auditory coin, 5 1% YOof the cases and having a heightened sense of of Officers reported getting tunnel vision in 51 for time distortions, officers experienced slow motion in 56% visual detail in 56%. As for 56% of of the shootings and fast motion in 23%. 23%. Finally, officers reported experiencing some “other” "other" shootings distortion 13% 13% of the time. Table 3 sets these figures in in relief with distortion statistics from • previous research to make for easy comparisons (where (where possible) with what other studies have 27 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. /( iI • reported about perceptual alterations during shootings. shootings. INSERT TABLE 3 ABOUT HERE of the Among the more interesting components of Table 3 is the graphic representation ofthe substantial variability in distortion rates across previous studies, which was mentioned in the literature review above. Most notable in this regard is that the officers in Artwohl and Christensen's (1997) (1 997) study were 3.6 times as likely to experience heightened visual detail than were the officers Solomon and Hom Horn (1986) (1 986) surveyed. Where the current data are concerned, frequencies for four of the six specific distortions measured -- tunnel vision, visual detail, •• 0 diminished sound, and slow motion -- fall within the range reported in previous previolls studies, studies. while the figures for the remaining two -- intensified sound and fast motion -- exceeded slightly the high previously reported. reported. With this comparative information in hand,9 hand,g attention now turns to a topic not considered in the previous research that addresses perceptual distortions during officerinvolved shootings; the temporal dimension. dimension. Officers in the current study experienced at least one of the several types of distortions distortions prior to firing their weapon in 88% of the shootings examined. The most frequently reported anomalies distortions were visual in nature, with officers experiencing one or both of the visual visualanomalies (78%) of the cases. They reported having a listed on the instrument in more that three-quarters (78%) • ( 1986) calculated overall rates for visual, auditory, and time distortions. They lhey 9 Solomon and Horn (1986) their.research vision, heightened visual reported that 56% of the officers participating in their. research experienced either tunnel vision. sorts of ofauditory detail, or both at some point during their shootings; 63% experienced one or both of the sorts auditory distortions; 82% for in the current data are 82% and 83% experienced one or both of the time distortions. The comparable figures 66% for time distortions. 85%'for visual, 85% for auditory. and 66% 28 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • heightened sense of visual acuity about some aspect of what they saw (e.g., the suspects weapon) in 37% of the cases, tunnel vision in 31 phenomt:;na in an additional 3 1% % of the others, and both phenomqna 10%. 10%. Officers' Officers’ auditory perceptions were altered in more than half half the cases, as they experienced a diminution of sound in 42% of the shootings and an intensification in 10% 10% (no officers reported experiencing both before firing). firing). Time distortions occurred in 55% 55% of the cases, with slow motion (43%) occurring far more often than fast motion (12%; (1 2%; again no one reported both phenomenon). Finally, officers reported experiencing some other sort of distortion prior to firing just 6% of the time. Officers experienced perceptual distortions at an even higher overall rate as they fired, reporting at least one in 94% of the cases. The occurrence frequencies for visual and time distortions at the time of firing were only slightly different from those observed prior to doing so e • 3 1% prior for tunnel vision, 35% vs. 37% for heightened detail, detail, 11 11% % vs. 10% 10% -- 27% during vs. 31 for both visual distortions; and 40% vs. 43% for slow motion, 17% 17% vs. 12% 12% for fast motion, and distortions. The rates for auditory distortions changed substantially 2% vs. 0% for both time distortions. from 42% to across the two time frames, however, as the rate of auditory blunting increased from figure for increased loudness was halved from 10% 10% to 5%, and the portion of cases in 70%, the figure 8%. Finally. the rate of aberrations rose from 0% to 8%. which officers experienced both auditory aberrations 6% to 9%. 9%. The statistics for distortions during both “other” distortions increased slightly, from 6% "other" time periods are presented in Table 4. Figures 11 through 6 present this information as a series of pie charts, each of which displays the percentage of cases in which officers experienced the o f distortions (i.e., visual, audito!). auditorq, and time) for specific manifestations of one of the three types of • tiring).. one of the two time periods (i.e., prior to or while firing) 29 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. Ii • INSERT TABLE 4 ABOUT HERE INSERT FIGURES 1 THROUGH 6 ABOUT HERE Temporal Variability in Distortion While the numbers regarding visual, time, and "other" “other” distortions reported above suggest that these perceptual anomalies are fairly stable during shootings, shootings, comparing what each officer experienced prior to pulling the trigger with the their perceptions while they fired discloses far more dynamism than indicated by the raw distortion rates. The case-by-case comparison . indicated that in some cases a given distortion (e.g., visual) began as officers fired, fired, while in others the same distortion, which began prior to firing, firing, ended when the officers fired their weapons. In other words, many of the perceptual changes between the two time periods canceled 1) • each other out in the aggregated data. Indeed, Indeed. a closer look at the data indicates that the degree of change in auditory anomalies anomalies is even more marked than the raw data suggest: suggest: officers reported diminished sound prior to firing in 42% of the cases vs. 70% while firing. firing. The case-by-case comparisons indicate that time distortions were the most consistent of the 113 1 13 cases. Officers across the two time frames measured, with concordance in 78 (69%) ofthe experienced no distortion at either point in time in 38 the cases, slow motion at both points in 32 in time distortions, the following cases, and fast motion at both in 8 others. For changes in 12 discordant cases in which officers experienced no differences were observed: Among the 12 distortions prior to shooting, 8 moved to slow motion and four 4 to fast motion as the officers 17 discordant cases in which officers experienced slow motion prior to fired. Among the 17 fired. • shooting, time returned to normal in 9 of them. moved to fast motion in 7, and was experienced 30 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • c in both slow and fast motion in 1 other as the officers officers fired. fired. Among the six discordant cases where officers experienced fast motion prior to firing, firing, time moved to slow motion in five and was experienced as both slow and fast in one other. other. Visual distortions were slightly less stable than temporal anomalies, as officers ocular experiences were consistent in 76 (67%) (67%) of the cases. Officers experienced normal vision throughout their shootings in 20 cases, tunnel vision both prior to and while firing firing in 22, heightened visual acuity at both time points in 28, and both tunnel vision and a heightened sense of detail detail at both points in time in 6 cases. The following changes in what officers experienced visually were observed: In three cases in which officers experienced no visual distortions prior to fired, while in two other cases officers discharging their weapons, tunnel vision set in as they fired, of visual detail as they fired." fired. 10 who had no distortion prior to firing had a heightened sense of • Among the 13 13 discordant cases where officers experienced tunnel vision prior to firing, the of detail in five tunnel effect gave way to normal vision in five cases and a heightened sense of others, while heightened visual acuity was added to the initial tunnel vision in three others. firing, the sense of of Among the 14 discordant cases that involved heightened visual detail prior to firing, detail disappeared in five cases, gave way to tunnel vision in six others, and was joined by the tunnel effect in three others. Finally, in the five discordant cases where officers had experienced firing, upon firing none moved exclusively to tunnel vision, the both visual anomalies prior to firing, tunnel aspect disappeared - leaving only heightened detail - in four others, and normal vision • e l10 o Among the more dramatic instances of heightened heightened visual detail comes from a case where two SWAT officers serving an arrest warrant warrant on a murder murder suspect simultaneously simultaneously fired as the suspect pointed a handgun at them. of his Both officers reported that they saw some of of their bullets strike the suspect, causing his shirt to pop-up off offofhis body with each successive hit. 31 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. e • returned in a single case. officers' perceptions remained stable only 53% of of the time (Le., (i.e., With auditory anomalies, officers’ in 60 cases). The most dramatic shift observed was from normal hearing prior to firing (which was the modal response; N=55) to diminished sound upon doing so, which happened in 34 cases. In four other cases, officers who reported normal hearing prior to firing experienced both of the time when reduced and increased auditory acuity as they fired. fired. In other words, 69% of officers experienced normal hearing prior to firing, firing, they experienced auditory blunting upon firing. firing. Information from the directed interviews offers additional insight about the nature of the remarkable attenuation of auditory acuity that so many officers (70% overall) experienced as they shot. shot. The vast majority of the officers who reported diminished sound upon firing (either by • itself itself or in concert with increased sound) indicated that it was their own gunshot(s) that was muted. Many officers who experienced this phenomenon reported that their shots sounded like a "cap “cap gun" gun” or "pop ”pop gun"; gun”; others stated that the gunshots simply were not as loud as they "should" “should” have been, been,”II and a small number of officers officers reported that they did not hear their rounds going off at all. The most extreme case of auditory distortion that included diminished sound involved a SWAT officer who fired a single single several-round burst from from a fully-automatic fully-automatic submachine gun at a barricaded gunman. gunman. He reported that while he did not hear his gunshots, he did hear the "clack“clack- a • II ” Officers who reported that their gunshots were muted typically indicated that they made this judgment measured against the numerous times they had heard the reports of shots they and other officers officers had fired on the police gun range and during other forms forms of training. Several of these officers expressed amazement that the shots shots they (and sometimes other officers) fired during confrontations with suspects were not very loud. loud. Among the more compelling of these stories concerns SWAT marksman who fired a single round from his sniper rifle at the same concerns a SWAT instant that his partner fired. Both officers were inside the same small room, yet the officer in question reported that soft. He reported that he knew that the sound should have been deafening and that the twin gunshots sounded quite soft. he can not understand why it wasn't. wasn’t. 32 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. Ii I • e clack" forward, ejecting spent clack” sound of the weapon cycling as the slide moved backwards and forward, casings casings and delivering fresh fresh rounds to the breech. 1 The The dramatic onset of auditory blunting in the form of diminished gunshots raises an interesting interesting question about the nature of perceptual distortions during police shootings: Does auditory auditory attenuation actually begin at the point when officers fire, or do they simply notice a phenomenon that had set in at an earlier point because they possess a marked auditory baseline of of what shots shots should sound like (from all the shooting they do in training) against which to compare what they provided they are are experiencing? If the first possibility is correct, the current research has provided some If it is the some interesting insight into how perceptual distortions arise during police shootings. If of distortion . latter, latter, on the other hand, hand, this would suggest that officers experience a greater degree of than they they consciously perceive and thus that the picture painted by the current data regarding the • as clear as it might first appear. timing of distortions is not as from another aspect of the data on perceptual distortions supports the Additional evidence from this latter possibility. As previously noted, officers reported experiencing some distortion(s) not among the six six sorts sorts specified on the research instrument prior to firing in 6% and upon firing in among 9% of the of the cases in the current study. study. Several of the officers reporting these “other” "other" 9% anomalies indicated that one of the "other" “other” distortions they experienced was a perceptual anomalies sense of distance, distance, where the actual distances between themselves, suspects, other distorted sense officers, citizen bystanders, and inanimate objects (e.g., vehicles) were either far greater or less officers. than they had perceived at the time of the shooting. The intriguing aspect of such reports is that officers’ realizations I-ttalizationsthat they had incorrectly perceived distances occurred during retrospective officers' • examination of their shootings where they learned of the actual distances between people and 33 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I I • objects when the shooting occurred. occurred. That is, only by viewing photographs of the shooting scene, of the scene (including measurements), participating in a postinvestigators' sketches of reviewing investigators’ shooting "walk-through"'2 “walk-through”” with investigators, investigators, or doing something else after the shooting to develop an understanding of the actual distances involved in their shooting, can officers come to know the accuracy of their understanding of the distances. Because many of the officers in the study did not later do anything that would inform them of the actual distances involved in their shootings, it is possible that officers' officers’ sense of distances is altered far more frequently than the current research suggests. Levels qf Distortion ofDistortion With the caveat in mind that officers may not always be aware of the sensory distortions I) • they experience, attention now turns from the matter of specific alterations to the issue of the overall degree of distortions officers experience during shootings. shootings. As was apparent in the above discussion of how frequently officers experienced visual, auditory, and time distortions, multiple sensory irregularities can occur in a single shooting. shooting. In order to tap this aspect of of the perceptual distortion picture in a parsimonious fashion, three scales were crafted that combine into single measures the legion of possible combinations of sensory alterations that may occur “distortion scales" scales” measures the overall incidents. The first of these "distortion over the course of shooting incidents. degree to which each officer experienced sensory alterations prior to firing firing in each of the 113 12 • I::' During investigations of police shootings, some agencies recreate the incident by having the involved officer(s) return to the scene after it has been processed to explain in detail what happened. Such a re-creation is “walk-through” because the officer(s) go over the shooting step-by-step, walking the investigators typically called a "walk-through" through the incident. 34 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • a shootings. It simply sums the number of specific distortions that each officer reported experiencing before firing firing in each shooting (each reported distortion coulIlts coupts for a single point). Because officers could have reported experiencing each of the six specific distortions listed in the instrument, plus a theoretically infinite number of "other" “other” alterations, the possible scores for the officers who reported no distortions prior to firing) firing) to a high of 6 scale range from a low of 0 (for officers + N (for officers who reported all six listed distortions, plus some number of "others"). “others”). The second distortion scale measures what officers experienced as they fired, and mirrors the first exactly. The third scale measures the overall degree of distortions experienced at any time during each shooting by simply summing the scores of the other two scales for each case. This overall measure of total distortion thus ranges from 0 (for officers who experienced no • 12 + N distortions at any point) to 12 N. INSERT TABLE 5 ABOUT HERE 5 , the observed upper limits of the scale scores --- five for prior, five As indicated in Table 5, firing, and 10 10 for total -- are quite modest in comparison to their infinite potential. This for firing, “other” distortions reported by any officer at any stems from the fact that the greatest number of "other" point in time was two. The fact that none of the officers scored a six on either the prior or during scales indicates that none of them experienced all of the six measured permutations of visual, auditory, and time distortions at either point in time. A closer look at Table 5 also indicates, on auditory, the other hand, that most officers did experience multiple distortions at each time point. Where • the time prior to firing is concerned, officers reported experiencing at least two distortions in 35 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I • e 70% just 11% 70% of the shootings, three or more in 37%, 37%, four or more in 6%, and five distortions in just % of the cases. These figures translate to a mean of of 2.02 distortions prior ty t9 firing per per shooting. The thatthey The degree degree to which officers experienced distortions was even greater during the time that they fired, fired, as as the average average number of distortions rose to 2.45 for this time frame. Officers reported at least two distortions while they were firing in more than three-fourths (76%) of of the cases, three sixth(l5%), or more in more than half (57%), (57%):four or five in more than a sixth (1 5%), and, finally, five distortions in four percent (4%) (4%) of the cases. Where experienced more Where the overall picture is concerned, Table 5 indicates that officers experienced (89%). This is than one one distortion during during the course course of nine out of every ten shootings (89%). understandable inasmuch as as the "overall" “overall” scale captures distortions that set in prior to the time that officers shoot, and then continue continue as as they fire, fire, in effect measuring a unitary phenomenon I) • twice. What What is interesting here, however, is that 82% of the time officers reported experiencing at least three distortions, which means that the vast majority of the observed multiple distortions in the overall scale are not due to the continuation of a single type of distortion across two time In sum, sum, the information information on officers' officers’ perceptions perceptions indicates that in most shootings officers points. In experience multiple perceptual anomalies during the course of the event. TABLE 6 ABOUT HERE INSERT TABLE Associations Among Distortions Associations Our understanding of the relationship between perceptual anomalies that officers Our experience during shootings shootings is enhanced by the information offered in Table 6, which presents experience • I2 specific distortions measured in the current study. zero-order correlations among the 12 study. The first 36 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. iI •e point of interest is is that the strongest positive correlations are between before and during measures measures of the same same distortions, distortions, as might be expected. The four largest coefficients are, in (r = = .61), .61), descending descending order, the the time one ane ane time two measures of heightened visual detail (r of tunnel vision vision (r (r == .50), .50), slow slow motion (r (r == .46), .46), and fast motion (r (r == .44). .44). A second point of interest is is that the relationships between these distortions at time one and time two are not particularly powerful, with just one zero-order correlation exceeding .50. A third point is that the weak bivariate bivariate associations associations between both sorts of auditory distortions across the two time frames (r ( r == .14 .I4 for for increased sound and .24 for diminished sound) reflect the previously noted instability of sound anomalies of diminished sound anomalies during shootings, particularly the remarkable onset of when officers begin to fire fire their weapons. These last two points together underscore the previous discussion about the instability of given distortions across time that is masked in the raw occurrence occurrence figures figures presented in Table 4. • The correlation matrix also shows that some different types of of distortions are related to one another, another, albeit modestly. Among the more notable findings in this connection is that when tojfiring, firing, they are more likely to experience auditory officers experience fast motion prior to , (r = .24) .24) and while (r (Y = amplification, both prior to (r amplification, = .25) firing. firing. Similarly, officers who us they theyJire experienced fast motion as fire are somewhat more likely to perceive an intensification of of (Y = .30) .30) and while (r (r = sound, both prior to (r sound, = .28) firing. firing. On a slightly different tack, when officers experience auditory blunting prior to firing, they are more likely to experience slow (r = .28) and while (r (r = = .24) firing. Other notable pairs that were likely to motion, both prior to (r following: tunnel vision and reduced sound while firing (r = = .29), occur in tandem include the following: • = .28), and fast motion prior to firing and slow motion and auditory blunting prior to firing (r = - 37 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. •a intensified sound while firing (r of anomalies are (r = .25). .25). On the flip side of the coin, some sorts of firing l for example, is less less likely to occur in the presence of others. Tunnel vision prior to firing, = -.38). somewhat less likely to occur in concert with increased visual acuity prior to firing (r = Likewise, = -.28), -.28), slow and fast motion prior Likewise, increased and reduced aural acuity prior to firing (r (r= to = -.3 -.31), to firing firing (r (r = = -.33), -.33), and slow and fast motion while firing (r (r = I), and tunnel vision and increased visual acuity while firing (r == -.27) are all a bit less likely to appear ttogether. ~ g e t h e 13r . ' ~ firing (r Multiple dimensional scaling was employed to obtain additional insight into the relationships relationships among the perceptual distortions officers experienced. The Alscal procedure employed, employed, which allows one to assess the degree to which binary variables cluster together, identified two two distinct dimensions dimensions within the distortions officers experienced and four distinct clusters of pairs of of the clusters of distortions on these two dimensions. Two of these clusters consisted of • same distortion at different times: visual detail and tunnel vision. These findings simply confirm same what what the the bivariate analysis analysis disclosed regarding the increased likelihood that officers will experience experience these distortions when they fire fire ifthey if they experienced them prior to pulling the trigger. The other two two clusters clusters were were a bit more intriguing. intriguing. The first first consisted of slow motion The to firing, firing, slow slow motion while firing, firing, and diminished sound prior to firing. This suggests that prior to there is is a dimension dimension of distortion distortion that has to do with an attenuation of temporal and aural there perception. The The other other cluster included the four distortions of fast motion both prior to and while perception. 15 The use use 13 The • of Pearson's r to estimate associations between binary indicators produces statistics that attenuate the the underlying underlying relationships relationships between variables. Additional analysis was undertaken in order to ensure that attenuate to measure measure the the bi-variate relationships between distortions did not produce a calculating zero-order zero-order correlations correlations to calculating inisleading picture picture of of which which pairs of distortions are are significantly related. In I n this exercise, each of the three pairs of misleading non-significantly related related variables variables that that were were on on the cusp of significance were cross-classified in order to estimate a non-significantly Phi statistic, statistic, which which produces significance significance tests that are are not based on attenuated relationships. This exercise found no Phi additional significant bi-variate relationships among the distortions additional significant 38 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. iI • e firing and increased sound both prior to and while firing. This suggests that there is another dimension of distortion that consists of a tendency for increased temporal and aural perception. Together, these two clusters suggest that aural and temporal perception tend to operate in concert, both increasing and decreasing together. Given the weak bivariate correlations between time and sound distortions, however, it must be kept in mind that this tendency for co-occurrence is not a strong one. INSERT TABLE 7 ABOUT HERE INSERT FIGURE 7 ABOUT HERE Sources ofDistortions of Distortions Some of the literature literature on perceptual distortions asserts that sensory alterations are the Some result of a rapid discharge of stress hormones (e.g., (e.g., adrenalin) adrenalin) that occurs when the sympathetic e • nervous system is activated by the brain's brain’s perception of an immediate life threat from from the environment (e.g., Grossman and Siddle, 1999). 1999). This argument suggests suggests that there should be links between the perceptual distortions distortions that officers experience experience during shootings shootings and both fear for for self and adrenalin rushes. In order to get some some empirical purchase purchase on this thesis, the relationships between all three of the perceptual distortion scales and the measures of fear for self and and strength/adrenalin strengtwadrenalin rush were were examined. The first step step in this process was to estimate estimate the zero-order correlations between the following prior to shooting following pairs of variables: fear for self selfprior shooting and all all three scales, scales, fear fear and all three distortion scales, scales, strength/adrenalin strengtwadrenalin rush prior to shooting and for for self while shooting and the scale that measured distortions while firing, firing, and strength/adrenalin strengtwadrenalin • 39 39 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. i • e rush 14 rush while while shooting shooting and and the the scale scale that measured distortions while firing. firing.I4 This This exercise exercise disclosed only thin support support for for the notion that distortions during officerinvolved involved shootings shootings emanate emanate from from physiological responses to threat. With alpha set at .05 .05 for the one-tailed one-tailed tests tests suggested suggested by the the hypothesis, just five five of the eight relationships examined were statistically statistically significant, significant, and and all all of these were quite weak. The largest correlation observed was between prior to prior distortions (r followed, in between fear fear for for self selfprior to shooting and andprior (r =.22). This was followed, descending totaZ distortion (r (r =.20), =.20), strength/adrenalin strengtwadrenalin rush while firing descending order, order, by prior fear fear and total and and distortions distortions during during (r (r= =..19), 19), strength/adrenalin strengtwadrenalin rush prior to firing and prior distortions ((rr =.16), =.16), and and fear fear for for self during and distortions during (r (r =.16).15 =.16).15 The The next step step taken in the the assessing the relationships between fear, adrenalin rushes, and perceptual distortion of examine whether experiencing both fear for self and a rush of distortion was to examine 0 • strengtwadrenalin in tandem was was more strongly associated with distortions than were either of strength/adrenalin of the phenomenon by themselves. themselves. This This was accomplished by creating two additive scales that the the degree degree fear/adrenalin fearladrenalin officers experienced prior to and while firing (for both scales 0 = tapped the fear nor adrenalin adrenalin rush, 1 == either fear or adrenaIin adrenalin rush, and 2 == both fear and adrenalin neither fear rush), and and regressing regressing the appropriate measures of distortion on these scales. This exercise rush), disclosed that the combined fear/adrenaIin feadadrenalin measures were more consistently correlated with disclosed distortion than were the separate separate measures, but that the associations were only marginally distortion I4Neither the the prior nor the total rota1 distortion scales were regressed on the measures of 14Neither of what officers experienced while shooting because the temporal sequencing would not be proper. Fear and strength/adrenal strength/adrenal in rushes experienced while shooting shooring can not affect officers officers perceptions before they tire; rushes fire; hence, the exclusion of of the while shooting shooting thoughts/feelings and prior distortions associations. Similarly, the relationships while relationships between fear and while firing firing and overall distortion were not estimated because the total distortion scale includes strength/adrenalin while strength/adrenalin tofiring it. firing in it. perceptions prior to • l5 15 same five five pairs of variables as being significantly associated. T-tests identify the same associated. 40 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. •e stronger than the significant associations observed in the first step of of the analysis. All four of of the associations plu~ fear/adrenalin while i.e., fear/adrenalin feadadrenalin prior with all three scales, plug associations examined -- i.e., firing fear/adrenal in prior and firing with distortions while firing -- were statistically significant, with feadadrenalin distortion fear/adrenalin prior and distortion prior having the largest correlation (r ( r =.28), =.28), followed by feadadrenalin overall (r =.26), =.26), fear/adrenalin feadadrenalin while firing and distortion while firing ((rr =.23), overall distortion firing firing (r and =.18). and fear/adrenalin feadadrenalin prior and distortion while firing (r ( r =. 18). In sum, sum, empirical consideration of the matter offers tepid support for the notion that the perceptual distortions that officers experience during shootings emanate from physiological responses to perceived threat. Looking at feeling of fear and strength/adrenalin strengtwadrenalin rushes separately disclosed disclosed that three of eight associations associations were not significant and that the five significant associations were quite quite weak. weak. When the two predictors were examined in tandem, the strength • of the associations associations between officers' officers’ relevant thoughts/feelings thoughtdfeelings and perceptual distortions were still the 16 quite quite modest. modest.I6 Recall Recall of of Shots Shots Fired The examination of officers' officers’ responses during shootings closes with ·consideration consideration of The of a on the the perceptual perceptual distortion theme. The literature includes the assertion variation on - but no supporting data data -- that police police officers officers often are are not aware of how many rounds they fire during supporting shootings (e.g., (e.g., Artwhol Artwhol and and Christensen, Christensen, 1997). 1997). The current data offers an opportunity to obtain shootings • I6It should be noted. however, however, that the binary nature of the fear and adrenaline measures suggests that bi16 1t should variate relationships between distortions and fear fear and adrenaline may be stronger than what was observed in the variate relationships between present analysis. Because the dichotomies used truncate the variation in fear and adrenaline, it is possible that more present analysis. Because the dichotomies sensitive indicators that more precisely measured these phenomenon would have produced stronger associations. associations. sensitive indicators that more in mind when making judgements about the degree of support that the analyses presented in Readers should keep this Readers should keep this in III ind making this report report lend lend to to the the fear/adrenaline-distortion fear/adrenaline-distortionhypothesis. this 41 41 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I i • e some some empirical empirical purchase purchase on this matter because it contains information on both the the number of shots shots officers officers actually fired fired (as (as determined by the the post-shooting investigat~on) investigation) and the the number of rounds they had believed they had fired fired at the time of the shooting. The The instrument included an officers to report the total total number of shots shots they fired fired during during each shooting. shooting. The The item that asked officers also asked during during the directed interviews interviews how many rounds they had believed they officers were also fired, fired, and this information was added to the data set. set. actually fired in 112 112 of the 113 1 13 Officers knew precisely the number of rounds they actually case, the officer could not remember whether he actually fired fired seven or shootings (in the other case, rounds). The The distribution of shots shots actually fired fired was highly skewed with aa mode of one (N (N eight rounds). = 33), 33), a mean of 4.5 = single uncertain case as as 7.5 7.5 rounds), and a high of 41. 41. (counting the single 10 rounds in 105 105 (93%) of the cases. Among the remaining eight, eight, Officers fired fewer than 10 • 14 rounds in one case, 15 15 rounds in one case, case, 16 16 rounds in officers fired 13 13 rounds in one case, 14 case, 18 18 rounds in one case, case, 28 rounds in two cases, and, and, finally, finally, 41 41 rounds in one case. case.”17 one case, beZieved they had officers provided on the number of rounds they believed The information the officers as precise. Officers reported that they believed they had fired a specific specific number of fired was not as 1 13 cases. cases. In 10 10 others, others, officers reported that they had believed that the shots in 90 of the 113 fell within a specific range (e.g., "I “I thought I fired 8 to 10 10 number of rounds they had fired fell rounds.”). Among the remaining 13 13 cases were two in which the officers had no firm firm numbers in rounds."). mind, but believed that they had fired more than the actual number they discharged; two in which discharged, but believed that they the officers did not have any firm idea of how many rounds the discharged, • l 7 The three cases in which officers fired more than 20 rounds transpired during large-scale SWAT SWAT 17 gunmen.. operations that involved protracted gun battles with barricaded gunmen 42 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I • e had had firedfewer than the the actual actual count; count; and nine in which the officers had no idea whatsoever about about how how many rounds they fired. fired. The The figures figures for number of rounds actually fired and the number believed to to have been fired fired are displayed in Table 8, with the case where an officer believed that he he had fired either seven or eight rounds counted as eight. INSERT INSERT TABLE 8 ABOUT HERE A A case-by-case case-by-case comparison of the number of rounds thought to have been fired and of shots was actually actually fired fired in each each of the 90 90 cases where officers recalled firing a specific number of conducted in in order to to round out the assessment of the degree of agreement between actual counts officers' and and officers' officers’ recollections of how many shots shots they fired. fired. This exercise disclosed that officers’ recollections turned out to be incorrect in 14 14 cases. Overall, then, officers could not accurately recall the exact exact number of rounds they fired in 37 (33%) (33%) of the shootings (i.e., the 14 in which • officers' officers‘ recollections of specific numbers were incorrect, plus the 10 10 in which officers 13 in which they had no firm numbers in mind). guesstimated a range, plus the 13 of the A closer look inside the data provides some additional highlights about the nature of discrepancies between officers' officers’ actions in pulling the trigger and their impressions of of this discrepancies 14 cases where the officers' officers’ belief behavior. Among the 14 behavior. belief that they had fired some specific shots was mistaken, the figure figure they had in mind was lower than the actual count in 12 number of shots cases and higher in the other 2 cases. cases. Among the ten cases where officers reported a specific cases range in which they believed the actual number of shots fired fell, fell, the true figure was within the range in three cases and higher in the other seven. Adding the two figures for low-count cases (i.e., 12 12 and 7) 7 )to the two cases where the only notion officers’ officers' had about the number of of rounds (i.e., • of 21 they fired was that they believed they had fired fewer than they actually did, yields a total of 43 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e cases in which the officer's officer’s recollection ofthe of the number of shots they fired was lower than the actual number. Adding the two high-count figures figures (i.e., (Le., two among the cases where officers' officers’ specific numbers and two among the cases where officers recalled a range) yields a total recalled specific fired was higher than the true number. of four cases in which the number officers believed they fired summarize to this point, among the 37 cases in which officers' officers’ could not accurately recall the To summarize 2 1, over-counted in 4, had no idea how number of rounds they fired, officers under-counted in 21, many rounds they fired in 9, and had in mind a range that included the actual number in 3. 3. Another point of information that emerges from a closer look at the data is that the officers’ recall tends to decrease as the number of shots they fire increases. None of accuracy of officers' 13 or more shots had an accurate understanding of the number; five the eight officers who fired 13 had no idea how many rounds they fired (including the three who fired 28 or more) and the other • 17 shootings shootings in which officers fired six to three thought they had fired fewer shots. Among the 17 nine rounds, the officers had a specific recall that was accurate in just five five cases. (They undercounted the number in seven, over counted in two, two. recalled a range within which the actual one). In the 88 cases where officers fired five or fewer number fell in two, and had no clue in one). rounds, the officers were correct in their belief belief about the number of rounds they fired in 71 7 1 cases. (Officers recalled a number that was lower than the actual count in 11 11 cases, recalled a higher (Officers number in 2, had no clue in 3, and recalled a range into which the actual number fired fell in the 81% remaining case). case). Thus does the recall accuracy rate drop from 81 % when officers fire five or fire 13 13 or more. fewer rounds, to 29% when they fire six to nine, to 0% when they fire figures on officers perceptions of Perhaps the most interesting single data point from the figures • the number of rounds fired is that in one case the involved officer was not aware that he had fired 44 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • any rounds at all. In this case, the officer in question was shot at close range by a suspect who then immediately fled the location. The suspect was captured nearby after losing a gun battle officer's partner. It was discovered during the post-shooting investigation that with the injured officer's the first officer had fired one round that struck the suspect during the initial confrontation in which himself had been shot. The first officer had no recollection that he had fired his gun.'' gun. 18 he himself Information gleaned from the directed interviews regarding the other 112 112 shootings disclosed a variation on the theme of not recalling firing firing one's weapon. In several of the cases in which officers' perceptions of the number of rounds they fired jibes with the number they actually shot, the officers stated that they were not aware of the fact that they were firing as they trigger. In such cases, the officers officers developed their understanding of the number were pulling the trigger. e • fired at some point after they ceased firing firing shots, shots. but before the incident had of rounds they fired ended. In one such instance, instance, the only reason the officer knew that he had fired four four rounds is that ended. shotgun, which he emptied at the suspect before he knew that he carried four rounds in his shotgun, drawing drawing his sidearm (which he did not fire). fire). He did not recall firing firing four four rounds; rounds; he just deduced from his empty shotgun that he had done done so. so. In another case, case, an officer knew that he had fired fired from semi-automatic handgun only because when he looked down the frame frame to obtain a sight his semi-automatic suspect who was wrestling with his partner, he saw that the hammer was picture on an armed suspect cocked back, back. which could only have happened if he had already fired, fired, because the gun he was fires the the first first round double-action and subsequent subsequent rounds single-action. single-action. At that point, carrying fires • 0 I s Another interesting interesting point from from this incident concerns concerns the the officer who who captured the gunman. gunman. He He received 18 the shootout that brought the suspect down, down, but was was not aware aware that he he had been been shot shot until until aa gunshot wound during the well after the was over. the incident was 45 45 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. / II • e and before the situation situation was resolved, the officer realized that he had fired a single round as he officer's partner rolled the suspect was was bringing the gun up from from his holster. It was only after the officer’s onto onto his his back that the officer knew that his round had found its mark. Because officers officers such as the two mentioned immediately above developed the correct understanding of the number of rounds they fired only because of some external cue, it is evident that the rate at which officers have independent cognition of of the number of of rounds they fire during during shootings shootings is is somewhat lower than the two-thirds figure reported above. Because the perception figures of their figures in some cases are measuring officers' officers’ retrospective understandings of actions actions (albeit very soon soon after the action in question), it must be understood that the hard data in Table 8 present a conservative picture of the scope of the deviation between officers’ officers' perceptions of and and the reality of how many rounds they fired. fired. • Summary of Findings Regarding Reactions During Shootings The following following points summarize summarize what the present data disclose about officers’ The officers' reactions during shootings: shootings: 1) 1 ) Officers experienced a wide variety of specific thoughts and feelings during during the encounters in which they shot others, 2) The thoughts and feelings that officers experienced often shifted during during the course of their shootings, shootings, 3) a large majority of officers experienced some sense sense of fear fear for for self, self, others, or both at some point during their shootings, 4) the vast some officers experience at least two types of perceptual distortions during majority of the time, officers shooting incidents, incidents. 5) 5 ) when comparisons were possible, the experiences of shooting of the officers in the fell within the bounds of what previous research has reported on current study generally fell 6) officers' officers’ perceptions can change substantially over the course of perceptual distortions, distortions, 6) of • shooting incidents, incidents, 7) some specific distortions are more likely to occur in tandem with others, shooting 46 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • while some are less likely, 8) the degree of of distortion that officers experience is mildly mildly related related to to feeling of fear and adrenalin rushes, and 9) there are substantial and systematic deviations between officers' of rounds they fired and the number number of of rounds officers’ understandings of the number of they actually fired. RESPONSES RESPONSES AFTER SHOOTINGS SHOOTINGS The instrument included several items that queried officers about the thoughts, emotions, and physical responses they experienced during four distinct time periods following their shootings: (1) within the first 24 hours after the shooting, (2) from the second to the seventh day, of the third month, and (3) (3) from the beginning of the second week after the shooting to the end of (4) (4) after three months had passed. For simplicity's simplicity’s sake, these time frames will henceforth be referred to to as as the first day, day, the first week, week. the first three months, and post-three months (and like • verbiage), verbiage), respectively. For each of these four time periods, officers were asked to report whether they experienced each of the following following psychological or emotional phenomena: • •a elation •a sadness sadness •a numbness numbness •a thoughts about the shooting recurrent thoughts •e fear for for their physical safety safety fear • fear of legal legal and/or administrative problems fear •e anxiety anxiety •a nightmares nightmares •a other thoughts/feelings thoughts/feelings other 47 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. •0 And each of the following physical responses: •a nausea •a loss of appetite •a headaches headaches •a fatigue fatigue a • crymg cryw •a fallinghtaying asleep asleep trouble falling/staying •a symptoms other physical symptoms / of 54 statements ~ Officers Officers were also also queried about whether they felt that each of a set of lifted lifted almost verbatim from from Campbell's (1992) ( 1 992) instrument -- dealing with post-shooting opinions and experiences experiences that could have occurred at any time after their shooting applied to them. • of their Presentation of what the study disclosed about officers' experiences in the wake of shootings will focus focus on the information information gleaned from from the time-specific items and use the shootings 54 statements statements to flesh-out specific points. Before moving forward with information from from these 54 information this material, material, however, however, aa brief discussion of the the timing of the interviews vis-a-vis the shootings is this in order. order. in Seven of the the 113 1 13 shootings took place less that three months before the involved officers Seven 12 days days prior, one occurred 19 19 days prior, and the other five sat for for their their interviews: interviews: one one occurred 12 sat 18 days prior. One other occurred between between two two months months and three days days and two months and 18 occurred shooting occurred three and one-half months before the involved officer was interviewed. These shooting eight cases cases present an an obvious obvious problem in a research project that sought to develop information eight • 48 48 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e about what officers experienced experienced since the three month mark following their shootings. shootings.’’19 It was resolved through the following following procedures: First, the officers involved in the two shooting that had occurred less than 20 days prior to the date of interview were instructed to ignore 'all a11 items that dealt with post-shooting responses after the first week. Second, Second, the officers involved in the other six shootings shootings in question were instructed to ignore the items dealing with responses after three months had passed. Third, based on the assumption that officers who had not experienced a given phenomenon within the first following a shooting shooting were not likely to experience experience an onset of that response two-plus months following within the next several days, the officers involved in the five shootings shootings that occurred between two and three months prior to sitting for interviews were instructed to respond to the items that dealt with post-shooting responses between eight days and three months. Finally, the various the • “not applicable" applicable” in the data set. items that these officers were instructed to ignore were entered as "not The research also included one case that presented a different sort of data collection problem. In this case, the subject officer suffered a life-threatening gunshot wound during the shooting in question (a firefight with an armed robber), flat-lined three times before medical personnel finally stabilized her, and was unconscious for most of the first 48 hours following following the shooting. Consequently, Consequently, the officer was instructed to ignore all items regarding responses during shooting. the initial 24-hour time frame. frame. These items were then coded as "not “not applicable" applicable” in the data set post-shooting responses began with the one week time for the case in question and entry of post-shooting frame. • ”) Two of the remaining 105 105 shootings occurred seven months prior to interview and the remaining 103 103 19 happened at least one year prior. The longest amount oftime of time between shooting and interview was 25 years. Six of the ‘80s. 1990s. the shootings occurred in the 1970s, 25 in the' 80s. and the remaining 82 in the 19905. 49 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • a 1 12 cases that include In the end, the above procedures yielded a data set with 112 following shootings,20 shootings,*' 113 1 13 cases information on officers' responses within the first 24 hours following 111 cases with information regarding the during the first week, 111 with information on responses during 105 that contain information about officers' responses after three months months, and 105 first three months, had passed. With this background information in hand, attention now turns to what analysis of the experiences, starting with their data disclosed about the subject officers' post-shooting experiences, responses. psychological and emotional responses. Psychological/Emotional Responses The first step taken to analyze officers' psychological and emotional responses was to determine the number of cases in which officers experienced each type at any anypoinf following determine point following 0 • 21 This process 1 13 shootings were examined. examined.2' their use of deadly force. For this exercise, all 113 disclosed that recurrent thoughts were by far the single thought/emotion most often experienced. Officers reported this response at some point following 96 of the shootings. The directed inter\ iews disclosed that few few of the officers viewed having recurring thoughts as as a negative interviews experience (see discussion of sadness below). Indeed, Indeed, most officers described the thoughts they experience had in either positive or neutral terms (see discussion of elation below), although many reported that they had second-guessed themselves, wondering if they had taken appropriate actions '" • ~o The officer who was told to ignore the items for the first 24 hours reported that she had suffered some decrease in the volume of oxygenated blood that reached her brain during the first several loss of memory due to a decrease communicating through minutes after being shot. After regaining consciousness, she was told that she had been communicating communication) with others during during the first two days post-shooting. physical gestures (intubation precluded verbal communication) has no no recollection of any such discussions, nor of anything else during this 48-hour time frame. She has ' I See See Table 9 for the same information on the 104 104 shootings for which full full data is is available. "' 50 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • a immediately prior to and during the moments they shot. shot.”22 One theme that several officers touched on in this connection was wondering if they could have done anything to avoid pulling the trigger, contemplating whether different actions might have led to some resolution short of force. Another related theme for some officers was how well they performed in terms of deadly force. appropriateness of the number of rounds firing their guns; some officers reported questioning the appropriateness they fired, while others reported trying to figure out why the bullets they fired did not always strike precisely where they had aimed (e.g., why rounds aimed at the suspect's suspect’s chest struck the suspect's suspect’s arm). arm). Anxiety Officers reported having a sense sense of anxiety at some point following following 48 of the shootings. shootings. Some of the officers officers who had this response reported that they were anxious anxious about the prospect of • shootings. One officer who was involved in a shoot-out fairly fairly soon after getting into additional shootings. graduating from from the police academy, academy, for example, example, figured figured that having a shooting early in his career meant that he might well be destined to be involved in others and was therefore anxious 23 about the prospect of future Other officers who experienced anxiety linked the future shootings. shootings.23 feeling to concerns about the prospect of sharing sharing what happened with others, others, press coverage, coverage, feeling shooting. The officers from from this second group group typically fell fell into and/or investigations into the shooting. shooting before and therefore therefore were anxious anxious categories: 1) those who had not been in a shooting one of two categories: the unknown, unknown, or 2) 2) those who had been in prior shootings shootings and were not looking forward to about the 77 “I reviewed the incident again and again, wondering if n-- Officers responded affirmatively to the statement, "I I did the right thing" thing” in 31% 3 1% of the cases. cases. • ’’ The officer's officer’s intuition was correct. correct. He was involved in a second shooting shooting six six months after the first. first. 23 51 51 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • telling others about another the incident, incident, going through another investigation, and so on. Concerns A bout Legal and Administrative Concerns About Administrative Repercussions Concerns about the post-shooting investigation were also salient for officers who reported fear that the shooting might create administrative and/or legal problems for them. Officers fears in 40 of the cases, worrying that they might be punished by their agency, sued reported such fears in civil court, and/or charged with a crime for shooting the people they shot. shot. For some officers, officers, such fears fears emanated from the shooting itself. Among the officers whose fears fears were rooted in what transpired during the shooting were two who shot unarmed citizens whom the officers believed were about to shoot them. Understandably, both officers were quite worried about how their agency and the legal system would respond after it was determined that the suspects were in fears of legal and/or administrative entanglement came from fact unarmed. For other officers, fears • shooting. In one such case, an officer who was initially confident things that occurred after the shooting. that he had acted properly became quite concerned about legal and administrative matters during the on-scene portion of the investigation into the incident. As he waited for the investigation to commence, his attorney (who came to the scene as part of the agency's standard post-shooting protocol) got into a heated argument with the homicide division supervisor who was in charge of the investigation. When the officer -- who was some distance away and thus could not hear what quarrel, he figured that there must be some big problem with the was being said -- saw the quarrel, shooting; why else would the two go toe-to-toe at the scene of an officer-involved shooting? As shooting; it turned out, the row between the detective-lieutenant and the attorney was over some other question. When the officer's attorney matter entirely, and had nothing to do with the shooting in question. • told him this, it calmed his fears, fears. but made him quite angry with both his attorney and the 52 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. /I • a lieutenant for airing their differences differences in a fashion that had caused him great consternation. Elation Officers experienced a sense of elation at some point following 33 of the shootings. The Officers directed interviews identified three types of elation among the officers who reported experiencing of joy about having survived a life-threatening it. The first sort that officers spoke of was a sense ofjoy situation. The officers who experienced this form of elation reported a profound satisfaction about being alive following an event that could have left them dead. The second type of elation reported is a form of exhilaration (most often in the first 24 hours) that appears to be a type of residual emotion from the sheer excitement of the situation where they fired. As one officer put it, he was "hyped-up" “hyped-up’’ for a while after his shooting. The third type of elation officers described -- • 0 which takes two distinct forms -- is deep satisfaction about doing their job properly. The first form of deep satisfaction was felt by officers who reported that they had wondered how they would perform if they were ever involved in what they described as the utmost challenge in a law enforcement career: an encounter where deadly force is necessary. These officers reported feeling elated that they had passed this ultimate test. The second form of job-related elation is social in nature, a deep satisfaction from proving to one's one’s peers that one is of law enforcement tasks. Most of the officers competent to handle themselves in the most trying ofJaw who reported this sort of elation reported that they had no doubts that they would perform well if situation. but nonetheless felt that they had to prove their mettle to confronted with a shooting situation, peer^.'^24 The rest of the officers who experienced this social form of elation felt a doubled their peers. • 24 Several male SWAT officers reported this sort of elation in terms of having proven their worth to their female patrol officer reported that her sense ofjob-related ofjob-related elation stemmed from the sense that she teammates. One female had proven to her male colleagues that she could handle herself herself properly in life-threatening situations. 53 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • 0 sense of accomplishment for having shown their competency to both themselves and their peers. from In sum, none of the various sorts of elation that officers reported involved pleasure taken from I hurting or killing the person they shot, shot. but rather described feelings of excitement about the event, joy about being alive, or accomplishment about doing a tough job properly. Sadness following 29 of the shootings. It was often Officers reported having a sense of sadness following over the fate of the person they shot, though not always always in relation to their injury per se. Several of the citizens shot by officers were, in the minds of the officers, tragic figures whose lives came to tragic ends. Some of the citizens. for example, example, were suicidal and chose to end (or try to end) gunfire,‘j while others were lost souls whose long-standing their lives in a hail of police gunfire,25 substance abuse had in some fashion led them to the deadly confrontation with authorities. The • emotions in terms officers who expressed sadness after shooting such citizens often framed their emotions of feeling bad that a fellow fellow human could devolve to such a state that they ended up on the wrong 26 Other officers expressed sadness for the members of the family end of police guns. guns.26 family of the person they shot, feeling sorry for them to have lost a loved one. Still other officers reported that they felt bad that they had been involved in a situation that scared their loved ones, causing their parents, spouses, or other family members to worry about their safety (in some cases because they had been injured). Finally, Finally. some officers reported feeling sad for innocent people who were 25 25 One officer who wounded an apparently suicidal individual reported a sense of sorrow for the citizen he citizen’s family maltreated their obviously disturbed relative by, among other things, refusing to shot because the citizen's permit him to accept a plea bargain that would have provided the mental health treatment he so clearly needed needed. For (commoiiI\ background information on the phenomenon of suicidal individuals seeking death via police gunfire (commonly a]., (1998). (1998). “suicide-by-cop” in law enforcement circles), see, for example. Hutson et a\., called "suicide-by-cop" • 26 26 10 cases officers responded affirmatively to the statement ..-‘I shot.” In IO ( felt sorry for the subject who was shot." 54 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. II • e injured or killed by the citizen they shot in the moments before they took the citizen under fire fire.. Numbness Officers reported a sense sense of numbness at some point after 23 23 of the shootings. They often described this numbness in terms of being so so overwhelmed by the shooting and its aftermath that they were mentally, emotionally, emotionally, and/or physically spent. spent. When asked to describe the numbness he was feeling, feeling, one officer stated that he felt as if "he “he had used up all of his brain cells" cells” dealing with all that had happened during the shooting, shooting, the events it had set in motion (e.g., the investigation), and the other post-shooting reactions he was experiencing. experiencing. Nightmares Officers Officers reported having nightmares after 20 of the shootings. shootings. In most cases these nightmares had themes that revolved around police shootings, while in some there was no a • event. Shooting-related nightmares took two forms. forms. The first sort in some apparent link to the event. fashion replayed the shooting incident (sometimes with different circumstances, characters, and/or outcomes). In one case, for example, an officer had repeated dreams of the thief she shot charging at her. The second sort of shooting-related nightmare officers reported consisted of dreams where the officer was involved in a different deadly force force encounter. encounter. In this type of dreams typically unable to defeat their opponent, either because their gun would wouId nightmare, officers were typically effect. Finally, in dreams with not properly function or because the bullets that did strike had no effect. no clear linkage to the shooting, shooting, officers often had visions of monsters and similar entities. entities.”27 27 27 • 11 cases officers officers agreed with the statement that after the shooting, "I “I had bad dreams dreams about things not In 11 shooting.” In some of these cases the officers officers did not report having nightmares among the specific related to the shooting." post-shooting reactions they experienced. When quizzed about this apparent discrepancy during the directed interviews, the officers in question indicated that they had similar dreams prior to the shooting and therefore did not attribute those they experienced afterwards to having been involved in a shooting. 55 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. Ii • for Safety Fear jor Officers felt some fear for their safety following 20 of the shootings. For most of the officers, the fear took the form of a realization that they could have been injured or killed during the incident where they fired. For other officers it was a fear of becoming involved in another incident, one which might not end as favorably for them as the shooting they survived. The fears fears that officers expressed typically had less to do with being afraid of injury and death and more to do with worry about what their loved ones would do if something bad happened to them. Perhaps the most sobering example of such fear arose in an officer when the person he shot (who had been holding a child hostage) was released from custody and began to stalk and threaten the family. The officer became quite fearful fearful that if he were incapacitated or killed officer and his family. 0 • that his family would be at the mercy of a dangerous lunatic. Guilt Officers experienced some sense of guilt for a variety of things in 14 14 of the cases. For example, one officer who shot an individual armed with a toy gun felt guilty over having hurt someone who posed no actual threat to him. In a related vein, some officers expressed guilt over having harmed or killed the person they shot, even though the individual had engaged in actual action. In one case such a sense of guilt carne came when the involved officer's officer’s father life-threatening action. dad’s shooting. The officer reported that for a while he felt that his dad's passed away soon after the shooting. shot. Other death was a form of punishment for him having taken the life of the gunman he shot. officers experienced guilt for not having done their job as they perceived they should have during • the shooting. shooting. One officer o f h e r who reported this sort of guilt felt badly that he was not able to prevent 56 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • the escape of any of the four bank robbers with whom he engaged in a wild shootout while 28 working an off-duty job by himself. himself.** I Miscellaneous Psychological/Emotional Psychological/Emotional Reactions Miscelluneous In addition to the several specific psychological/emotional psychological/emotional responses responses that officers reported experiencing, they indicated that they had some "other" “other” thought or feeling in 48 cases. miscellaneous reactions was anger, which officers The most commonly expressed of these miscellaneous 15 cases. The object of the anger was often the person they shot. shot. In some such cases reported in 15 officers were angry at their opponent for trying to kill them, while in others they were upset at their target for forcing them to shoot. One officer, for example, example, was angry at the suicidal citizen he shot for involving him in a demented death drama. Other officers were upset with fellow fellow officers over actions the later took during shooting incidents. One such officer -- who shot a • @ dozens of other officers had failed to do so during a tense stand-off stand-off -rifle-toting gunman after dozens expressed anger at the officers who did nothing because he felt that at least one of them should have shot the gunman long before the subject officer arrived on the scene. Still other officers were upset with the detectives and other law enforcement officials (e.g., district attorney personnel) over some aspect of the fashion fashion which they investigated and reviewed the shooting. Finally, some officers expressed anger at the news media for what they viewed as biased shooting that put them in a bad light. Some of the officers who received "bad “bad coverage of the shooting press” shootings singled-out for special reprobation specific reporters who they felt press" about their shootings had been especially biased, offering withering, bitingly negative, comments about them during •a 28 The officer did strike one of the robbers with several of the rounds he fired. The suspect escaped only because the body armor he was wearing prevented incapacitating injuries, allowing him to scramble into the get19. away car. This is the same case first mentioned on page 19. 27 57 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. ( • e the directed interviews. Half a dozen officers reported a sense of pride or satisfaction over1the overitheactions they took during the shooting. This response is, obviously, closely related to the satisfaction-based elation discussed above. above. Information collected during the directed interviews suggests suggests that the difference between the two types of responses is one of the nature of satisfaction or pride felt. felt. “elation” response category The officers who felt satisfaction or pride and who checked the "elation" seemed to have had a more visceral response compared with their peers who chose to check the “other” category to register their more cerebral sense of accomplishment. Whatever the case "other" “other” category indicates that more officers experience some might be, the information from the "other" officers’ responses to the measure of satisfaction from their actions than was indicated by officers' "elation" “elation” item. a • “other” thoughts or feelings feelings that officers reported were a few Counted among the "other" responses that could be viewed as the flip side of the job satisfaction coin. One officer, for example, reported a sense of disappointment that the shots she fired did not strike her opponent where she had aimed. while another said he was embarrassed about his inability to appropriately shot: an emotionally disturbed individual armed assess the actual threat posed by the person he shot: from other with a toy gun. Other responses officers reported included the desire to withdraw from people, the desire to avoid confrontations, an increase in the frequency of benign dreams, secondfocusing on tasks such as reading, guessing the decision to make police work a career, difficulty focusing feel bad about and wondering if there was something wrong with them because they did not feel • 58 58 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. (I • e killing another human. human.”29 Finally, one "other" “other” reaction that was reported in 3 cases deserves deserves special attention: depression. In two of the cases the depression struck well after the one week mark, but before three months had passed, and lingered well into the post three month period. In the third case, the depression struck well after the three month mark. During the directed interviews. the officers involved in all three shootings shootings reported that their symptoms included a reduced enthusiasm for life. One officer reported that this component of the depression depression got so severe that he contemplated contemplated suicide. Shortly after hitting this low point, the officer sought counseling from a mental health professional who was able to help him regain his desire to live. Table 9 below summarizes summarizes the psychological and emotional responses that officers shootings by providing frequency distributions for experienced at any point in the aftermath of shootings • @ 1 13 cases and for the 104 cases with missing data for one or each specific response among all 113 frame. The table clearly shows that the proportion of cases in which officers more time frame. samples, with experienced a given response changes only negligibly between the full and reduced samples, most responses occurring in the same proportion in both samples and the largest changes a mere three percent, which occurred with just two variables. • reported no ill 29 Only one officer reported this response on the instrument. Several other officers who reported of their the'ir interviews. effects from their shootings, however, raised this issue in the form of a question at or near the end of These officers (often those who reported a sense of satisfaction after the event) asked the interviewer if he believed that there was something wrong with them because they had not suffered any notable negative repercussions from having shot someone. The interviewer asked them why they thought this might be the case. Most ooff them stated that they had received training. training, or through some other means had heard, that officers invariably have a hard time in the aftermath of of a shooting. shooting, that it was normal to have a negative response, and that ipsofucto ipso facto they must not be nonnal. questionnaire, reasoning that they The rest indicated that they derived this impression from having filled-out the questionnaire. if the federal government was sponsoring research that were supposed to have had some negative repercussions if of the officers in the later category actually apologized obviously focused on negative responses to shootings. Some of of the officers who expressed concern about interviewer for having so few negative responses to report. All of to the interviewer their positive or neutral responses were relieved when the interviewer opined that there was absolutely nothing jeopardy. wrong with such reactions to shooting someone whose actions had placed innocents in jeopardy. 59 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • INSERT TABLE 9 ABOUT HERE I Physical Responses With a sketch of officers' officers’ post-shooting thoughts and feelings in hand, attention now turns to what the current data discloses about their physical responses. As was the case regarding psychological and emotional responses, responses, presentation of this information begins with a discussion of how frequently frequently officers experienced each sort considered at any point following the shootings. shootings. The most commonly reported physical response was trouble sleeping, which officers experienced following 55 55 of the shootings. shootings.”3D Logically enough, the next most frequently frequently experienced response was fatigue, with officers reporting being tired in 52 cases. Officers reported that they cried at some point following 27 of the shootings, experienced a noticeable decline in their desire • 19 cases, got headaches in 8 cases, cases. felt nauseated in 5, and reported experiencing for food in 19 some other physical reaction in 21. 2 1. “other” physical reactions were a diverse bunch with no single type reported in These "other" more than a handful of cases. Officers experienced elevated levels of energy in 5 cases. cases. In two “added” and "excess" “excess” energy to describe this phenomenon, such cases officers used the terms "added" “adrenalin rushes" rushes” (in one case while in the others the officers reported that they experienced "adrenalin only upon recounting the shooting to others). others). In three other cases officers reported trembling or shooting. Two of these cases involved patrol officers, one shaking at some point following the shooting. 30 Sleep problems included phenomena such as simply not being able to fall asleep, sleeping and waking in fits, and waking-up in cold sweats. One officer reported that the sweats he experienced were so seVere severe that starts and fits, after waking he would have to strip and re-sheet his bed. take a shower, and put on fresh fiesh his sleep clothes before seeking more slumber. slumber. 3D • e 60 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. ( • e where the officer began to shake while being interviewed by the detectives who investigated the shooting, shooting, and the other where the shakes came as the officer told his wife about the shooting. shooting. I The third case involved a SWAT marksman who began to tremble at the conclusion ofthe of the first call-up after the shooting in question, an incident in which he had killed an armed murder suspect. Just one other response was reported in more than one case: an increase in appetite, which occurred in two cases. Among the responses reported in a single case were a marked increase in sex drive, increased alertness, bouts of diarrhea, and a compulsion to exercise, which the officer in question indulged with a vengeance. vengeance. The several physical reactions officers reported are summarized in Table 10. 10. As was the case with thoughts/feelings, frequencies are presented for all 113 1 13 cases as well as for the 104104case sub sample. Again, the information indicates that the proportion of cases in which officers • experienced a given response does not vary markedly between the full and reduced samples. 10 ABOUT HERE INSERT TABLE 10 Taken together, the data on psychological/emotional and physical reactions indicate that recurrent thoughts about the event are far and away the most common post-shooting response officers’ minds in the wake of more among the officers studied. When such thoughts entered officers' (83%), than eight of every ten shootings (83 %), none of the other specific responses were experienced in even 50% of the cases, and only four ofthem of them -- trouble sleeping, fatigue, anxiety, and fear of 1egaVadministrativeproblems -- occurred more than one-third of the time. legal/administrative • Comptrrison With With Previous Research Comparison 61 61 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I • officers’ post-shooting responses To get some idea of how the preceding information on officers' figures were compared with the fits with what has been reported in previous research, the figures numbers reported in extant studies. The perspective offered by this exercise was limited by the nature of the data presented in the other research because the previous studies employed a variety findings derived from common items in a variety of ways. None of of instruments and reported findings studies, for example, measured all ofthe of the post-shooting responses included in the instrument the studies, used in the current research; one (Artwhol and Christensen, Christensen, 1997) 1997) reported no data whatsoever, al., 1984) reported no information on specific responses in a form that and another (Stratton, et aI., could be translated into the percent of cases where they occurred. occurred. The nature of the extant research thus renders impossible a thorough response-by-response comparison across studies. studies. What is possible is a limited comparison, which at least affords some consideration of how the • current data stacks up with what previous research has disclosed about officers' officers’ post-shooting responses. responses. Tables 11 relief data from current and previous studies. 1 1 and 12 12 use all 113 113 cases to set in relief studies. feelings, comparisons are possible onjust on just four of the nine specific response For thoughts and feelings, categories used in the current study. What these comparisons show is that officers in the present study experienced recurrent thoughts at a higher rate than did officers in the other studies, anxiety, less frequently frequently felt guilt, and were less likely to have nightmares. experienced more anxiety, Where physical reactions are concerned, the other studies provided information in a fashion that allows for comparisons for four of the six specific responses measured in the current study. For one of these, nausea, the two previous studies that provided data present strikingly different • pictures of how often officers suffer this reaction. At one end of the continuum lies Campbell 62 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • a (1992), (1 992), who reported that just 1% YOof the FBI agents he interviewed reported feeling nauseated. At the other end lies Nielsen (1981), 10 officers (198 l), who reported that more than 9 out of every 10 (92%) in his study experienced "nausea/upset “nausedupset stomach" stomach” following their shootings. shootings. Given this range, it is not surprising that the 4% nausea rate ra’te among the officers who participated in the current study falls falls with the range reported in previous research. Where the other three responses that can be compared are concerned, concerned, officers in the current study were much more likely to report fatigue fatigue than were the officers in the study with the next highest fatigue rate (46% vs. the 24% reported by Campbell), very slightly more likely to report having trouble sleeping than the officers in the study with the next-highest rate (48% vs. the high of 46% reported by Solomon and Horn, 1986), and slightly more likely to suffer headaches than were the officers in the study Campbell’s 5%), but far less likely than the officers in the with the lowest headache rate (7% vs. Campbell's • study with the highest rate (Nielsen, 1981, 1981, who reported that 25% of the officers he studied experienced headaches in the wake of their shootings). shootings). 11 AND 12 12 ABOUT HERE INSERT TABLES 11 Overall, this limited comparative analysis of how officers in the current and previous Overall, studies responded in the wake of their shootings suggests that the pattern of post-shooting reactions among the officers who participated in the present study is not wildly at odds with what previous research has reported. In other words, the initial look at current data paints a picture that is consistent with what previous studies presented; that a large portion of the time officers • who shoot citizens experience some notable post-shooting reactions. It must be kept in mind, 63 63 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • however, that this first officers' postfirst cut at the data is quite coarse because it simply examined officers’ shooting responses as officers' as a whole. whole. A closer look at the current data that examines officers’ responses responses over time, however, however, presents a substantially different picture from the one that emerged from from the initial analysis. Temporal TemDoral Variability in Post-Shooting Responses Tables Tables 13 13 and 14 14 present the percentage distributions of cases where officers experienced each of the several of the four postseveral emotional/psychological emotional/psychological and physical responses during each of shooting shooting time periods considered in this study. study. Perhaps the most striking information conveyed by these tables is is a strong strong tendency for the proportion of cases in which officers experience a given response 51 possible adjacent time comparisons response to diminish diminish as time passes. Across the 51 (i.e., first = 3 first day day to first first week, first week to three months, three months to post three months = • comparisons 5 l), the figures figures drop in 43 of them. are equal in 5 others, comparisons x 17 17 response categories = 51), and increase increase by a single single percentage point in the other 3. By the time three months have passed, and moreover. the proportion of cases in which officers experienced given reactions decreased by at moreover, 50% in 16 16 of the 17 17 response categories,3! categories,” with 12 12 of the 16 16 falling by at least two-thirds. least 50% 13 AND 14 14 ABOUT HERE INSERT TABLES 13 Tables 13 13 and 14 14 also show that the temporal decrease is so pronounced that by the threeTables few of the responses were manifest in even 10% of of the cases. month post-shooting mark very few Only one one specific specific reaction -- recurrent thoughts -- persisted in more than one-third of Only of the cases, • 31 The single single category that didn't didn’t drop by by at least 50% 50% -- fear for self self - decreased slightly, from from 9% 9% to 8%. 8%. 64 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. /I I 1I • and only two others broke the 10% 10% mark -- fear of legal problems and trouble sleeping -- both of which were reported in 11 11% YOof the cases. cases. Because, Because, as noted above, the directed interviews disclosed that few of the officers who reported recurrent thoughts defined them as negative, the percentage distributions clearly indicate indicate that specific specific negative post-shooting reactions were quite rare after three months had passed. These low rates strongly suggest that only a small proportion of the officers officers interviewed suffered suffered any remarkable long-term detrimental consequences from the shootings in which they were involved. involved. The notion that officers tend to suffer some notable post-shooting reactions in the short term but little disruption in the long run is supported supported by a more sophisticated look at the data that uses additive scales to measure the negative effects of shootings shootings at each of the four time periods considered in this study. study. These scales were crafted by simply summing the scores of the 13 13 0 • response categories that represent negative reactions, reactions, plus any "other" “other” reactions -- such as anger and depression -- that could be construed as negative, negative, for each time period and for each case. case.”32 Because officers could have experienced experienced each of the 13 13 specific specific negative responses, responses, plus a theoretically theoretically infinite number of "other" “other” negative reactions, the possible scores for the scale range frame) to a from a low of 0 (for officers who reported no negative responses for each given time frame) 13 + N (for officers who reported all specific reactions, plus some number of "others"). “others”). high of 13 INSERT TABLE 15 15 ABOUT HERE Table 15 15 displays the means and frequency frequency distributions for each of the four post- scales. Comparisons of the scale means across all four time frames shows a shooting adjustment scales. 32 32 • 0 Recurrent thoughts and elation were both excluded from these scales, the former because so many of the thoughts officers reported had no negative connotation, the latter because elation is not a negative emotion. Similarly, positive or neutral "other" “other” responses (e.g., (e.g., pride) were not counted in the scales. 65 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • 0 marked drop in the average number of negative responses officers reported as time passes, from 2.88 in the first 24 hours, to 2.05 in the first week, .77 week. to 1.06 within three months, and finally to .77 by the time three months had passed. Two points stand out in this pattern. One is that the mean of nearly 3 in the initial 24-hour period indicates that the shootings typically led to some notable short-term disruptions disruptions for the study officers. The second is that the disruptions abated substantially over time. Two other aspects of the data in Table 15 15 confirm this second point: 1) the high score drops from 12 12 in the first first 24 hours to 8 after three months have passed and 2) the proportion of cases where officers reported no negative responses rose three-fold over the course of three months, from 21% during the first day to 63% at the post-three month mark. In sum, the data from the post-shooting scales clearly show that while involvement in shootings typically led to some notable negative short-term reactions, the vast majority of the officers experienced very • from their shootings. shootings. The scope of these changes over time is little or no long term fall-out from graphically presented in the line graph presented in Figure 8. 8. INSERT FIGURE 8 ABOUT HERE Insights From the Directed interviews Interviews The directed interviews shed substantial light on why post-shooting responses, including those that are positive and neutral, typically diminished so markedly over time. The next several paragraphs detail what the in-depth discussions with the officers disclosed on this point, beginning with the most frequently reported response: recurrent thoughts. thoughts. Nearly all of the officers who experienced recurrent thoughts during the first day and week following their shootings shootings reported that their ruminations occurred largely in relation to • queries from third parties. Police shootings nearly always prompt major criminal and 66 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e administrative investigations and typically raise the curiosity of the involved officers' officers’ superiors, peers, family, friends, friends, the press and, sometimes, members ofthe of the community at large (e.g., Geller and Scott, 1992). 1992). Many of the officers in the current study reported that they were well aware that third parties would be keenly interested in their shootings, and that their knowledge of this prompted them to dwell on the shooting in the immediate aftermath. Their minds were then in family, and other officers fact directed to the shooting by the numerous queries from friends, family, (including those conducting investigations), press coverage, and, in a few cases, community outcry about it. As time passed and others queried them less frequently (and press coverage and community outcry died down in those cases where notable coverage and outcry occurred), most of the study officers thought about their shooting less and less. Thus, by the time three months had passed since their shooting, fewer than four in ten officers reported experiencing recurrent • thoughts. thoughts. In sum, the directed interviews indicated that the degree to which officers ruminated shootings is influenced substantially by third party reactions to the event. about their shootings officers’ fears fears The directed interviews also divulged a link between social reactions and officers' about legal/administrative problems, which helps to explain why the proportion of cases in which officers felt such consternation declined over time. Most of the officers who reported being worried about being indicted, disciplined by their agency, and/or sued immediately after their shootings indicated that they felt this way because they had heard of (or knew) other officers shootings who had suffered such fates. In most cases, as the post-shooting investigation moved forward and it became apparent that they would suffer no legal or administrative repercussions, the officers’ fears fears were allayed. allayed. In a few cases, on the other hand, the post-shooting investigation officers' • a officers’ concerns. In these cases, some aspect of the investigative only severed to heighten officers' 67 67 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. i/ II •e process (particularly investigators' if they were investigators’ questions) typically led officers to feel as if suspected of having done something wrong (see discussion of aggravation below for a related theme). theme). Concerns Concerns about legal/administrative repercussions continued for some officers until their department department and the criminal criminal justice system cleared them of any wrong doing. In many cases these clearances came within a few few days, days, in others it took several weeks, while in others still it took more more than three months. As more and more of the shootings were ruled justified as time passed, fewer and fewer officers experienced fear that they might suffer some legal/administrative of legal and legal/administrative problem. Thus an understanding of the temporal pacing of administrative administrative investigations into police shootings helps explain the monotonic decrease in the portion of cases cases where officers reported fear of legal/administrative problems (see page 78 for • additional additional discussion of the relationship between litigation and post-shooting responses). In a related vein, feelings feelings of anxiety typically died down as time passed because the matters that worried officers officers played themselves out over time. time. For officers who were anxious matters about the the investigation investigation into into their shooting, shooting, the investigations were typically completed in about relatively short short order. order. For officers officers who were anxious in the immediate aftermath of relatively of their shootings about the the possibility of being involved in another one in the near future, the anxiety shootings waned as as additional additional shootings shootings typically did not come to pass. In sum, sum, as time passed and the waned concerns that that prompted their anxiety were either resolved or faded faded as time passed, officers concerns became less less anxious. anxious. became Social reactions to to shootings also also go go a long way towards explaining why so many officers Social • experienced sleep sleep disruption disruption and and fatigue fatigue during the first 24 hours after their shootings, as well as experienced 68 68 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e why the proportion of officers experiencing these responses rapidly diminished as time passed. Many of the officers reported during the directed interviews that the investigation into their shootings extended into the time when they would normally be sleeping, that discussions with (e.g., peers, friends, friends, and family) kept them occupied into some portion of the time other people (e.g.. sleep,33 or both. With their normal nonnal sleep cycle disrupted in the first 24 that they would normally sleep,” hours after the shooting, many officers had trouble falling and/or staying asleep when they finally did get to bed. For some officers, the initial disruption bled into the next day or two, which helps frame. After the first week account for some sleep problems reported during the week one time frame. had passed entirely, which coincidentally corresponds with the waning of immediate third-party interest, the majority of the officers who had some initial difficulty sleeping got back to their nonnal routine . normal slumber routine. • a Where fatigue fatigue is concerned, concerned, the pattern of initial sleep difficulties followed followed by stabilization helps to explain the decrease in the rate of languor over time. Logically enough, officers who experienced sleep disruptions in the first 24 hours following their shootings tended down, their fatigue fatigue tended to to be tired. As officers got the rest they needed when things calmed down, dissipate. The directed interviews interviews also indicated that third-party responses had a more direct link dissipate. fatigue officers officers experienced. experienced. Several ofthe of the officers who complained of short-tenn short-term with the fatigue fatigue reported that they were simply worn out from the process of repeatedly recounting the fatigue investigators, friends, friends, family, family, and so so on. o\er o\ er the course of several hours. hours. shooting to peers, investigators, reactlon: The directed interviews also disclosed a social component to another physical reaction: • a 33 example, reported that his phone kept ringing and his pager keep going off for for se\eral seieral 33 One officer, for example. hours after the investigation into his shoo! ing was completed. shooting 69 69 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e crymg. crying. The officers who cried typically did so when they told loved ones about the 34 For most of these circumstances circumstances of their shooting, usually within 24 hours of the shooting. ~hooting.’~ officers the tears came when they recounted the event to their spouses, for others when they told other family members. The thoughts and emotions emotions related to such crying often revolved around concern that the officers felt for their loved ones. Some Some officers, for example, example, were upset that the shooting shooting had increased the fear that their spouses spouses felt regarding the officers' officers’ occupation. In a different vein, one officer who cried when telling his mother and father that he had been in a shooting shooting reported that he did so because he felt sorrow for his parents. When quizzed about why he felt sorry s o w for them, the officer replied that he felt bad that his parents had to know that their son was a killer. killer.”35 One other post-shooting response that has a clear social component is elation. As • experienced this emotion felt it in the context of discussed earlier, many of the officers who experienced having proved to others that they could acquit themselves properly in a crisis setting. setting. For some came. logically of these officers the understanding that others felt that they had done a good job came. 36 shooting.j6 enough, when others told them that they had done a good job, often soon after their shooting. enough, 34 One poignant exception to this occurred when an officer oficer broke down and cried just a few moments after he seriously wounding a shotgun-toting suspect. The officer was the lead man on a search warrant team when h~ confronted the suspect in a back bedroom. As the suspect began to swing his weapon toward the officer. the officer suspect, the officer in question walked ualked to fired, striking the gunman in the arm. As soon as other officers secured the suspect, the front of the house, sat down on the front porch, and began to weep uncontrollably. 34 35 Additional evidence that discussing the shootings shootings they were involved in prompts some officers to shed study. Several officers officers broke down and cried as they tears comes from the directed interviews conducted for this study. related their stories to the PI. 35 36 One poignant example of the effect of third party praise comes from a case where the officer shot a woinaii and her two children hostage inside their home. The officer rep0l1ed reported that the gunman who was holding a woman sense of satisfaction he felt over over- having saved the three lives was deepened further when he received a letter ti·om from the woman’s husband praising him him for his actions actions and thanking him for saving the lives of ofhis woman's his wife and children 36 • 70 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I1 II • e As of elation typically waned. !In As time time passed and officers typically got fewer kudos, their sense of n sum, sum, the directed interviews indicated that officers' officers’ post-shooting reactions were influenced I substantially by what transpired in the aftermath of shooting incidents. A Deeper Look at Social Social Influence on Post-Shooting Adjustment The The current study also produced quantitative evidence that what transpires after the smoke smoke has has cleared affects affects officers' officers’ post-shooting adjustment. The instrument used in the current of the post-shooting social milieu research included a variety of items that tapped several aspects of that relevant literature identifies as factors officers' post-shooting adjustment. factors that can affect officers’ Previous Previous research (e.g.. Artwohl and Christensen, 1997; 1997; Solomon and Horn, 1986) has suggested that criticism from from third parties can make matters worse for officers who shoot, support from • has a palliative effect. talking about the shooting and its aftermath with others is positive, others has others actions by third parties that aggravate officers can be negative, civil litigation is harmful. taking actions some time off after shootings can help one's one’s adjustment, killing citizens is more disruptive than some them, and counseling sessions with mental health professionals are helpful. The wounding them, instrument included items that allowed for the development of indicators that afforded the opportunity to examine examine each of these hypotheses. To wit: opportunity •e A set of dichotomies measuring whether specific categories of of people expressed criticism about the actions the officers took during the shootings. •e Two sets sets of dichotomies measuring the support that specific categories of of people Two extended to the officers. • • A set of dichotomies that measured the degree to which officers discussed their shootings 71 71 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. i( • with specific specific categories of people. •a A set of dichotomies dichotomies that measured whether a variety of individuals and entities said or did anything in relation to the shooting that aggravated the officers. •a A dichotomy that measured whether the officer, hisher his/her agency, or both were named as defendants defendants in any civil action related to the shooting. • A dichotomy that measured whether the officer took any (non-punitive) time off immediately after the shooting. • 0 A dichotomy that measured whether any suspects died. • A dichotomy that measured whether the officer attended any mandatory counseling sessions with a mental health professional. With alpha set at .05 .05 for the one-tailed tests indicated by each hypothesis, assessment of • the bi-variate associations between these several measures and each of the four post-shooting post-shooting scales disclosed that officers' mental, emotional. and physical responses to involvement involvement in shootings shootings are indeed related to what occurs in their aftermath, but not always always as hypothesized. Criticism Criticism The instrument included items that queried officers on whether fellow officers, superior officers, family members, members, and non-police friends friends had criticized them about their actions in the shooting incident. Such criticism was rare, coming from from fellow officers in 13 13 cases. from superior officers in 10, the 10, from from friends in 7, and from family family members in just 2. Because of of'the separation problem that obtains when working with exceptionally highly skewed distributions, distributions. the relationships between criticism from family members and officers' post-shooting responses • were not examined. examined. The instrument did not ask officers about the timing of any criticism that 72 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e might have come from their friends, friends, peers, or superiors, but the directed interviews indicated that among those cases where such opinions were expressed, they were typically delivered during the lI first few days following tollowing shootings. Because the timing of the criticism creates a temporal sequencing problem vis-a-vis officers' officers’ responses during the first two post-shooting time periods, the relationships between criticism and responses during the first day and week were not examined. What remained to be assessed were the relationships between criticism from friends, friends, peers, and superior officers, on the one hand, and officers' officers’ responses during the one week to three month and post-three month time periods, on the other. The sole significant association among the six considered was a weak one between officers' officers’ reactions prior to the three month mark and criticism from fellow officers (r (r =.20). Thus the data indicates that criticism from fellow officers is associated with a mild elevation in the degree of negative reaction officers 0 • experience in the short term, but not in the long run. and that criticism from friends and supervisory officers bears neither any short nor long term relationship with officers' officers’ reactions. reactions.”37 Support The instrument also included two sorts of items that measured the degree of support that others offered officers following shootings. shootings. One type queried officers about whether each of the following categories of people offered words of support to the officer: fellow officers, superior officers, friends, and family members The other sort of item asked officers whether each ofthe of the “provided you with substantial support following this shooting": shooting”: following categories of people "provided their spouse, boyfriend, or girlfriend, girlfriend, other family members, fellow officers, supervisors, superLisors, or any • 0 37 37 rhe criticism measures were skewed, skewed. the scores for each case in which officers were criticized Because the set. if iftlie noteworth! patterns. the skew was masking a relationship. This exercise disclosed no noteworthy patterns. were examined to sec 73 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. (i • 0 other person. The data indicate that words of support were almost always offered by fellow (94%), while they officers (in cases), superior officers (96%), (96%), and family members (94%), (in 99% of the cases), II were notably less (just 80% of of the time). As for “substantial "substantial iess likely to be proffered by friends (just of the cases, their support," support,” officers officers felt felt that fellow fellow officers offered this level of support in 90% of spouses, 58%, spouses, girlfriends, or boyfriends did so so in 70%, other family members did so in 58%, %, and others did in 18%.38 supervisory supervisory officers did so so in 51 5 1%, 18%.j8 , As was the case with criticism, the instrument did not ask officers about the timing of of any statements statements of support were made or when "substantial “substantial support" support” may have been offered. The directed interviews indicated that while supportive statements were often offered within a few days days of the the shootings, shootings, they also also came well after the first week had passed. Keeping in mind the caveat caveat that that temporal order is is not clear-cut for the one-week to three-month time period, the a, .' associations between statements of support and officers' officers‘ reactions after one week and three months months were examined. Assessment of the links between officers' officers’ responses and supportive statements from from superiors, superiors, friends, friends, and family39 family’9 disclosed that support from friends were not statements associated with officers' officers’ responses responses during during either of the time periods considered, that such support associated from supervisors supervisors was was not associated with responses prior to three months but it was (albeit from weakly) afterwards afterwards (r (Y = = -.20), and and that support from family members bore a weak association weakly) with officers' officers’ responses responses both prior to to (r (Y = -.23) and after the three month mark (r ( r = -.-. 24). with The picture picture regarding the the role that "substantial “substantial support" support” plays in officers' officers‘ post-shooting The 58 An item item on on support s~ipportfrom from clergy was folded into the "other" ”other” category for analysis because less than a 38 An handful of of officers officers reported reported receiving receiving such such support .. handful •* 39 39 Because there there isis essentially essentially no no variability in \ierbal verbal indications of support support from fellow officers, ofticers, the Because potential effect effect of of fellow fellow ofticers' ofticers‘ supportive supportive statements was as not considered. potential 74 74 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I • @ adjustment is also mixed. Because the instrument sought no information about the temporal basis for officers judgements about the degree of support proffered by others, others. the time order of this second measure of support and officers' officers’ responses is problematic. Because of this, consideration of the associations between the degree of support offered by the various parties considered and officers' officers’ responses was limited to the one week to three month and post-three frames. The results of the several bi-variate models that were estimated indicate the month time frames. following: 1) the degree of support from spouses, boyfriends, or girlfriends is not associated with following: post shooting responses, 2) neither is the degree of support from other family members, 3) (Y = = substantial support from supervisors is a weak correlate of negative responses after one week (r -.20) ,, but not after three months, 4) substantial support from fellow officers bears a mild (Y = -.27) and after three months association with lower levels of negative responses both prior to (r • (Y = -.24), -.24). and 5) 5 ) support from others is not associated with any reduction in negative post(r 41J shooting reactions. reactions.’” support from third parties In sum. the empirical assessment of the association between support officers’ post-shooting reactions offers a mixed bag of evidence regarding the relationship and officers' between the two phenomena. While displays of support by some third parties are associated with a reduction in the degree of negative responses that officers experience at some points following shootings, the significant associations are not particularly strong, they do not hold across all time frames for all categories of people, and support from some sorts of people bear no association whatsoever with officers' officers’ post-shooting adjustment. adjustment. •* ptrzzling result is that words of support from "others" “others” is associated with inaeased imwnsed levels of 40 Another puzzling (r = .23). negative responses after three months (r 75 75 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • Talking With With Others Concerning detailed discussions about shootings, the instrument asked officers whether they had discussed the shooting in detail with their spouse, girlfriend, or boyfriend, other family members, fellow officers, superior officers, and other people. Officers had detailed discussions with their spouses, boyfriend, or girlfriend in 85% of the cases, other family members in 76%, fellow officers in 92%, 92%, supervisors in 69%, and other folk in 32%.41 32%.41 Ignoring the first two time periods due to the now familiar concerns about temporal sequencing, computation of the zeroorder correlations between each of these five scales for the five categories categories of people and the response scales last two time periods disclosed the following: following: 1) talking in detail with one's one’s significant other about the shooting was not related to post-shooting responses, 2) detailed discussions with other family members was not related to post-shooting responses, 3) talking with superior officers officers was 0 • associated with a slight reduction in the degree of negative responses during both the one week to three month and post three month time periods (r (Y = - .24 .24 for both), 4) discussing in detail the shooting with fellow officers was associated with a modest reduction in the degree of negative responses during durins both time periods considered (r (r = -- .36 .36 after one week and rt- = = - .35 2.5 after three months),41 5 ) detailed conversations with other third parties was not associated associated with a months),“ and 5) ‘IS Ihe tlie case with "substantial “substantial support," support.” the tlie instrument included a clergy clergq item iteni regarding discussions 4141 As \VilS that was folded folded inll. i n t o the "other" “other” category for for analysis. analysis. 42 41 Additi\lnal discussing the shooting with other officers comes from the Additional evidence for the positive effect of otdiscussing • 0 officers’ respllllses responses to two statements included in 1he the instrument. Fifty-eight percent perceiit of the officers agreed with study officers' ” I t h.:lped Iiclped me to to talk with other officers \\~ hoh had o been involved in shooting shooting incidents." incidents,” and 65% the statement. "It statement, "It “It helped me to share share experiences and feelings with others others who had been involved in agreed with the stalelllent, shootings." shootings.“ 76 76 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e officers' negative re~ponses.~’ responses. 43 Thus the data discloses that discussions with some reduction in officers’ of people are associated with some reduction in the degree of of negative reactions that categories of officers expenence, while discussions with others are not. Aggravation The instrument asked officers whether any of the following categories of people and entities caused them aggravation related to the shooting: fellow officers (they did in 14% 14% of the cases), supervisors (yes in 22%), politicians (yes in 3%), 3%), non-law enforcement friends (yes in 7%), prosecutor's office (yes in 4%), 7%), news media (yes in 30%), prosecutor’s 4%), suspect's suspect’s attorney (yes in 20%), suspect’s friends friends and/or family (yes in 12%), 12%), and any other person or entity (yes in 12%). 12%). The suspect's directed interviews indicated that when officers felt aggravation, it was typically about specific specific action they took. The following following examples words that some individual said or some specific • @ felt aggravated: aggravated: illustrate the sorts of cases where officers felt •e An officer reported being quite aggravated with one of his peers who was present at the repeated]). showing showing up at his calls for no legitimate reason in the shooting in question for repeatedly weeks following following the incident. incident. •e furious with a deputy district attorney who possessed aspirations for for An officer was furious “playing politics" politics‘‘ with his shooting by repeatedly discussing it in a higher office for "playing negative light with members of the press. press. • 0 Several officers were upset by some some aspect of the formal formal investigation into into their Several shootings. Among these cases cases was was one one in which the the officer was was peeved when when one one of the the shootings. • 0 J:1 4 however, there there are are slight and and modest increases increases in in negative reactions after after one one week (r (r == .19) .19) and and Again. however, ( r == .33), .-33), respectively. three months (r three 77 77 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I/ iI • detectives accused him of being less than forthright forthright about the number of rounds he fired because the detective was unaware that many SWAT officers do not carry fully-loaded fully-loaded clips in their assault rifles,44 and another where the officer felt he was being judged too harshly by his department's department’s shooting review board when they criticized him for not using Spanish when he ordered an English-speaking English-speaking Hispanic suspect to "drop “drop the knife" knife” before he fired. fired. • Several Several officers were incensed with particular journalists and/or news organizations for grossly misrepresenting pertinent facts facts about their shootings shootings on the air, in print, or both. • A few of the officers who checked the "other" “other” category were aggrieved with their spouses 45 for failing to offer them the support they felt that they deserved. deserved.45 Estimation of the zero-order correlations correlations between the several sources of aggravation and 0 • officers’ responses during the later two time frames frames disclosed the following: following: 1) Aggravation from officers' prosecutor's office was associated with higher levels of negative peers, superior officers, and the prosecutor‘s responses after both one week and three months. For aggravation with fellow officers, officers, r == .44 at one week plus and rr == .27 after three months; months; for superior officers the figures are .37 3 7 and .34, 34, respectively, and for the prosecutor's prosecutor‘s office they are .42 and .40 respectively. respectively. 2) Aggravation “other” sources was associated with higher levels of negative responses from the news media and "other" 4 44 The officer in question carried ait standard 30 round clip in his assault rifle, but. like many SW AT officers, SWAT 38 bullets in it in order to relieve the pressure on the spring that pushed fresh ammunition into the breech only kept 28 the tiring firing cycle. The investigator had never heard of this practice and thus thought that the officer must have during thc admittins to. fired two more rounds than he was admitting 1’. .,> Three male officers officers in particular expressed great anger at their wives for the way they reacted to the • e spouses were antagonistic towards them, even to the point of shooting. In these cases the officers reported that their spouses telling tht: the officers ofticers that they should have figured fisured out some way of resolving the situation without shooting anyone. anyone. “1 was disappointed with my All three of ofthese these officers (along with nine others) agreed with the statement that, "I spouse/bo\ spouse!bo\ 'girlfriends ‘girlfriends reaction to the incidcnt.'· incident.“ 78 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • after three months, but not before; with three month-plus correlations of.22 of .22 and .27, respectively. 3) Aggravation with suspects' officers' suspects’ attorneys and non-police friends was not associated with officers’ II 46 either time interva1. interval.46 responses at eIther Time Off Off Time Officers took non-punitive department mandated time away from work following 69 of the cases. Because the days off were taken during the first first week following the shootings, the associations between time off and post-shooting reactions were estimated for the one week to three month and post-three month time frames only. only. The zero-order correlations correlations indicate that taking some time off is associated with a mild reduction in negative reactions prior to the three month mark (r (r = = .18) . l S ) but that it is not associated with long-term reactions. Civil Litigation • e Fourteen of the cases resulted in civil litigation wherein the officer, the officers' officers’ department, or both were named as defendants. defendants. Because such litigation was typically filed and adjudicated well after three months have passed, the only litigation-reaction link assessed was that between the post-three month response scale and whether a civil claim was filed (a 0-1 dichotomy). Contrary to expectations. expectations, there was no relationship between civil litigation dichotomy). emanating from the shooting and officers' officers’ long-term post-shooting adjustment. a d j ~ s t m e n47 t . ~ ~ 4h The link between responses and agpiavation aggravation with politicians was not estimated because so few officers reported being aggravated with politicians. -II> 47 • a n One interesting aspect of the civil action picture concerns the relationship between litigation and fear of legal, administrative ‘idministrative problems. Among the 14 14 cases m e s tthat h d t did result in civil claims. claims, officers officers reported harboring fears of of legaL ‘idministrative problems at some point foll\lwing following the shooting in just six. six. By the time three months had passed. legal administrative this number had dropped to just three. Another thing for tor readers to keep in mind is that civil litigation could have iri those cases in which the interviews weie been tiled after data collection was completed ill were conducted within three the shootings. years of ofthe 79 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. (I/ • Mental Health Services More striking still was the finding that attending department-mandated meetings with , I mental health professionals (MHPs) in the wake of shootings was not associated with officers' officers’ “spoken reactions. The instrument included an item that asked the officers whether they had "spoken with a mental health professional (e.g., psychologist, psychiatrist) about .the the shooting?" shooting?” The response categories were "yes, so", "yes, “yes, my department required me to do SO”, “yes, I did so on my own", own”, , and "no:" “no.“ Officers were instructed to check all categories that applied to them. Officers spoke with no MHP in 16 of the cases, attended only mandatory sessions in 79, attended only sessions with a counselor they sought on their own in 4, and sought out additional counseling after attending mandatory sessions in the remaining 14 14 cases. cases. A measure of mandatory counseling was crafted by joining the 79 cases in which the sole sessions attended were mandatory with the • 14 in which officers sought subsequent sessions on their own, yielding ,aa dichotomy with a 14 93 cases where officers attended mandatory meetings and 20 where they did not. distribution of 93 Because the mandatory sessions that officers attended typically took place during the first seven following shootings, shootings, consideration of the associations between mandatory counseling days following officers’ reactions was limited to the one week to three month and post-three month sessions and officers' time periods. There were no significant differences in response scores between those cases where officers attended mandatary meetings with MHPs and those whcre wh~re they did not at either time period (r (Y = .06 and -.02 for pre- and post-three months, respectivdy). respectivLsly). Because the null officers‘ unexpl"cted, the issue of officers' findings regarding the effects of mental health counseling were unexpccted, interactions with MHPs will be revisited in some depth in the concluding section of this report. • 5,'uspect Sirspect Injury 80 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. (I • e All four post-shooting post-shooting scales scales were regressed on a dichotomous dichotomous measure of whether the shooting led to the death of any suspects 1 13 cases). cases). None suspects (at least one suspect suspect died in 65 of the 113 of the associations associations was significant. significant. Previous Shootings Shootings Thirty-three Thirty-three of the shootings shootings in the present sample involved officers officers who had previously shot someone someone during their careers in law enforcement. enforcement. This aspect of the data set affords affords an opportunity opportunity to examine whether post-shooting post-shooting adjustment adjustment following following a given shooting is affected affected by involvement in previous shootings. shootings. It could be that there is a cumulative cumulative effect of shootings shootings so that negative responses following following subsequent shootings shootings are exacerbated by having been ones. On the other hand, it could be that involvement in previous shootings shootings involved in previous ones. post-shooting response serves to reduce the degree of negative post-shooting response in subsequent subsequent shootings because • officers are familiar familiar with these events and related post-shooting assess officers post-shooting procedures. procedures. In order to assess evidence of either sort of effect in the present data. data. the sample was first divided whether there is evidence shootings that were an officers officers first (or only) and shootings shootings by an officer that had been into shootings involved involved in a previous previous shooting. shooting. Each of the four post-shooting scales were then regressed on the binary measure created by the sample subdivision subdivision and the findings findings from these bi-variate models were then checked with ANOV A. The analysis ANOVA. analysis disclosed disclosed no significant significant differences differences between the “groups” of shootings shootings during during the first day and first week follnwing following shootings, shootings, but that two "groups" officers who had been in previous shootings shootings experienced experienced slightly slight]! higher levels of distress distress after officers tirst "\leek cieek (r ( r =.24) =.24) and after after three months (r (Y = = .23). 23). These findings findings suggest that shootings shootings the first ma! officers’ longer term post-shooting adjustment, adjustment, but that may ha\ e a cumulative effect on officers' • 81 81 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. e • 48 involvement in previous shootings shootings does does not exert effects on short term adjustment. adj~stment.~~ THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN REACTIONS REACTIONS AFTER AND DURING SHOOTINGS SHOOTINGS Scholars Scholars who study how people react to involvement in traumatic events events have recently begun to investigate the notion that post-event responses are influenced by perceptual distortions during them. Using the term "peritraumatic “peritraumatic dissociation" dissociation” to describe alterations in perception during traumatic events, these researchers have assessed the relationship between perceptual distortions and post-event responses in populations such as combat veterans (e.g., Marmar et aI., al., 1994), crime victims (e.g., Griffin et aI., al., 1997), 1997), and emergency service personnel who responded to mass disasters (e.g., Weiss et a!., al., 1995). 1995). These studies have consistently found found a link between dissociative reactions and post-ewnt post-e\ ent responses, with subjects subjects who experience higher levels of • e distortion tending to have more problems in the wake of traumatic episodes. The measures of perceptual distortions and post-shooting responses in the current data afford an opportunity to investigate whether peritraumatic dissociation and post-event adjustment are likewise associated among police pol ice officers who shoot citizens. The first step in executing this opportunity was to estimate the bi hi-variate -variate relationships between the three distortion measures (i.e., prior to firing, firing, upon firing, firing, and aiid overall) and the post-shooting scales for each of the four time periods. With alpha set at .05 . 0 5 for one-tailed tests, just 2 of the 12 12 correlations were occurred as officers ofiicers significant. Both of these were \\M cak and both pertained to distortions that occUlTed significant. nere nced higher levels of "vere firing: firing: when officers experic experiL'nced of distortion upon pulling the trigger. they tended to suffer from slightly higlier higller levels of negative responses in the first tirst day and week • 48 48 The same sort of of analysis wit w;, ' conducted with the three distortion scales to assess whether previous previolls ctiootings ing subsequent ones. There were no significant differences in level\ levels of of shootings affected officers’ officers' reactions du dUll1g officers' first and subsequent ones. distortions experienced during shooting that were officers’ ~ 82 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e following the shooting (r (r = = .18 and .17, respectively). The second step in the investigation into the relationship between distortion and postshooting response was to examine the zero-order relationships between the three distortion scales and each of the 13 13 specific negative post-shooting responses that were measured in the current study. This exercise disclosed several significant associations across the four time periods (with alpha set at .05 .05 for one-tailed tests). tests). During the first 24 hours, three responses were correlated with at least one of the distortion measures: appetite loss (r (r = = .16 upon firing firing and .17 for total distortion), fatigue (prior, = .18, .18; upon firing, firing, r = = .23, and total, rY = = .23), and sadness (upon (prior, r = firing, rY =.21). During the first week, there were four negative responses that were significantly associated with perceptual distortions distortions during shootings: appetite loss (r (r == .16 upon firing), fatigue (r (Y =.18 =.18 upon firing firing and r =.16 =. 16 total), numbness (r (Y =.16 =. 16 upon firing and r =.17 =. 17 total). and a • sadness (prior, rY = = .25, upon firing, firing. r = .27, .27, and total, r = = .29). .29). Between the one week and three month mark, the sole significant association was between fatigue and distortions while firing (r = firing (r .19). Finally, after three months had passed. .19). passed, both sadness and guilt were significantly related with .23. while all three distortion measures. The correlations for sadness were as follows: prior, rr = .23, firing, r = = .21, and total, rY = .25. 2 5 . For guilt, guilt. the statistics were r = = .24 for prior, r = = .18 while firing, and rY == .24 for total distortion. distortion."49 The final step in the examination of the link between dissociative reactions during • i i m of events, the likelihood that officers would cry at certain time periods 49 In another unexpected lurn distortion they experienced during shootings increased. The likelihood of crying in the decreased as the degree of distortioll following a shooting was lower when officers experienced more distortions prior to firing (r ( r == -.22). - 2 2 ) . The first week following one \\eek \\eel\ and three months post-shooting dropped as officers' during and total scale likelihood of crying between olle ( F -.23 - .23 and - .20. 2 0 . respectively). scores increased (r= 83 83 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • shootings and post-shooting adjustment was to look at the relationships between distortion and the two specific responses that were not defined as negative: elation and recurrent thoughts. (because there is no expectation of direction), this With alpha set at .05 .05 for two-tailed tests (because exercise disclosed that both responses bore short-term significant relationships with all three distortion scales. When officers experienced higher levels of distortion prior to shooting, as they shot, and overall, they were more likely to experience elation during both the first 24 hours and the rest of the initial week, but not afterwards. The first-day correlations between elation and distortion were as follows: prior, r = = .28, .28, during, r =.25 ,, and total, rY =.30. The statistics for the first week were rr == .23, 040, .40, and .36, 2 6 , respectively. Where recurrent thoughts are concerned, concerned: significant associations were observed only during the first week, with prior, rY =.21, during, during. r • =.24, and total, r= .25. traumatic Some of the research that examines the link between distortions during traumatic episodes and post-event adjustment has also examined the possibility that feelings of fear for afterwards (e.g., Griffin et aI., al., one's safety during such events might lead to increased difficulties afterwards 1997). In order to round out consideration of the relationship between officers' reactions during 1997). for self and negative shootings and their post-shooting adjustment, the association between fear for examined. Three Three indicators of fear fear for for self wer~ ivere post-shooting responses in the current data was examined. exercise: the two dichotomies that measured fear prior to and while firing, plus a;I used in this exercise: llsed from the two binary measures (i.e., 0 = = no fear, fear, 1 == tear tealthree-step scale that summed the scores from .05 for for one-taikJ one-tailcd either prior to or while firing. and 2 = fear at both times). With alpha set at .05 • e tests, zero-order correlations between these three fear measures measiires and the four post-shooting post-shooting scall's 4c.alt.s tests. 84 84 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. II I1 • following: 1) All three fear measures were associated with mild to moderately disclosed the following: scores on the first day post-shooting response response scale (r (r = = .37 3 7 for prior fear, rY =.26 =.26 for fear higher scores .35 for total fear). fear). 2) The strength of the associations between fear and while firing, and r == .35 (to .38 .38 for prior fear, fear, .37 .37 for fear while negative reactions increased slightly during the first week (to firing, and .41 .41 for total fear), fear), then dropped after that (during the one week to three month time firing, . I 9 for prior fear, fear, .20 for fear while firing, firing, and .22 for total fear, fear, while only prior fear frame rr = .19 frame bore a significant significant association with the post-three month response scale [r [Y = = .17]). In sum, sum, shootings and negative reactions after them assessment of the relationship between fear during shootings disclosed a link between the two that was modest during the first 24 hours, grew in strength a bit day, and then declined markedly as time passed. after the first day, officers’ The following points summarize what the present data disclose about officers' • shootings: 1) officers experienced a wide variety of psychological, experiences in the wake of shootings: emotional. and physical reactions to being involved in shootings, 2) 2) the single most commonly emotional, experienced reaction was recurrent thoughts, thoughts. which officers experienced following following more than shootings, 3) 3) no other single single response was reported in even half of the cases, 4) eight out of ten shootings, of the shootings led to some notable short-term disruption in the involved officers' officers’ while most ofthe as time passed, 5) officers' officers’ postlives, in most cases negative reactions dissipated substantially as lives, shooting reactions are influenced by how third parties respond to shootings shootings events, and 6) 6) shooting officers’ post-shooting reactions are related to the thoughts, thoughts, feelings, feelings, and physical reactions they officers' experience during shooting events. With the above information on officers' officers’ responses after shootings shootings in in hand, attention now • a turns to a discussion that summarizes suinmarizes the findings findings of this study. study. highlights some of the policy turns 85 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • implications if if it, and points out some needs for additional research on officers’ officers' reactions to involvement in shootings. DISCUSSION, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS The preceding pages contain numerous findings about a variety of topics regarding police officers’ officers' responses to involvement in shootings. Perhaps the single most salient point among them is that the act of shooting another human being typically did not produce lasting disruption officers studied. studied. It is indeed remarkable that the officers' officers’ involved in more in the lives of the officers half of the shootings than half shootings reported absolutely no negative psychological, emotional, or physical responses after one week had passed since the incident and that the percentage of cases in which officers were reaction-free increased to nearly two-thirds at the three month mark. Additional analysis undertaken to gain some sense of how the current data fits fits with what previous research • has reported about officers' officers’ long-term adjustment places this key point in a comparative context. As noted above, two previous studies reported on the long-term effects of shootings on police officers. Stratton et al. (1984) and Solomon and Horn (1986) used different criteria for classifying the severi severity officers’ reactions, yet both reported that a similar proportion of classifying ty of officers' problems . Stratton et al. al. reported that 31 31% officers experienced notable long-term problems. % of the shootings hey were involved in had a substantial substantial longdeputies they queried indicated that the shootings term impact on them. while Solomon and Horn reported that 28% of the officers in their study had "severe" “severe” 10ng-tenTI long-tenm reactions. Following the logic that thLitSolomon and Horn used to judge the 011 page pagc 86 86 for details details of their measurement severity of post-shooting reactions (see discussion on ha1 cc‘ had a "severe" “severe” long-term reaction scheme), officers in the current study were deemed to hm • a t u o or more negative responses aller alter three months had passed since their when they experienced two 86 86 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. 0 • shootings. Analysis disclosed that 19% 19% of the cases meet these criteria. With fewer than one in shootings. five shootings producing "severe" “severe” long-term reactions, it is evident that officers in the current study were far less likely to suffer protracted problems than were their peers who participated in research. 50 previous research.’” Why this is so is a question worthy of consideration. consideration. One possibility is that the answer lies in the composition composition of the current sample, which includes a disproportionate number of SWAT officers. S WAT officers, one might reason, would be less likely to suffer serious SWAT serious longterm problems following shootings because they have a more aggressive, action oriented, outlook than does the average officer. In order to investigate the possibility that the over-sample of SWAT officers in the current study accounts for the low rate of severe long-term reactions, the (N = 65) was rate of severe responses among cases involving officers with SWAT experience (N • compared to the rate for those that involved non-SWAT officers (N = 40). There was no SWAT-cop shootings resulted in significant difference betneen between the two groups; 20.0% of the SWAT-cop two or more negative reactions after three months, while the figure for non-SWAT shootings was (2= .21 .2 1 where a score of 1.65 1.65 or greater would indicate a significant difference in the 18.4% (2 18.4% predicted direction). direction). The data thus indicate that the presence of a disproportionate number of SWAT officers does not account for the low frequency of severe responses in the present research.” research. 51 rcsponses Another possible explanation for the markedly lower rate of severe long-term responses 50 . 50 C Coiinting ountmg Im:e or more negative . Ilong-term ' .as."severe" h . reactions “severe” sows shows dramirtlc difference tthrce ong-term reactIons an even more dram(llll' previous rewarch. “severe” response rate ofjust 10%. 10%. between the current and previolls research. It yields a "severe" • 0 51 . 51 Time 65 cii\c> ca,c~ where the four scale scores here were also regressed on a dummy variable that differentiated the 65 S U AT ZT experience from the 40 others. others. The result: rr === -- .003. involved officer had S\\ 87 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I • in the current data is is that it is is an artifact of differences differences between the present and and previous studies studies in instrumentation and other aspects of the methods used to gauge officers' officers’ long-term adjustment to involvement in shootings. A brief discussion of how Stratton et a1. al. (1984) (1 984) and Solomon and Horn (1986) (1 986) classified the severity of officers' officers’ reactions details details why this might be so. so. Stratton et a1. al. provide absolutely no information about the post-shooting time frame frame to which their usage of the phrase "long-term" “long-term” applies. applies. They simply present the phrase in the title of a table that includes the percentage distribution of deputies' deputies’ responses to a single single five-point Likert-type item that asked them to rate the degree to which the shooting affected them. Stratton et al. al. then combined the two highest response categories to yield the 30% 30% long-term severity figure figure reported above. above. It IS is thus clear that the method that Stratton et a1. al. used to classify the severity of long-term reactions 0 • has virtually nothing in common with the one used in the current research. officers’ long-term reactions to to The method the current research used to gauge officers' involvement in shootings shares substantially more in common with the one used by bq Solomon and Hom Horn (1986), (1986), for both base their severity ratings on the number of reactions officers experienced after three months had passed since their shootings. A closer look at precisely how Solomon and Hom Horn classified officers' officers‘ reactions as severe, however, indicates that thal the methods used in the two erity rates are not as compatible as they might first appear. studies to develop se\ severity appear. Solonion Solomon and Horn based their sekct-ity sevcrity rating on a combination of 1) I) officers’ officers' responses to 15 tike-step live-step Likerttype items that quericd queri,:d officers about the degree to which they experienced speci tic negative • post-shooting reactions and 2) the length of time that ofticers ng each officers reported experieiici experiencing @ 88 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • a reaction. Officers of Officers were deemed to have had a severe reaction when their scores on 2 or more of the the 15 15 items items exceeded three and they indicated that the response persisted at this level after three months of reactions months had passed since since the the shooting. shooting.”52 Because the current study used the presence of during of the strength and during a specific time frame frame (i.e., (i.e.ythree-plus months), as opposed to ratings of duration duration of reactions, it is clear that there are substantial differences in the methods the two studies studies used to classify classifjr the severity of officers' officers’ responses. It It is is apparent from from the above above information that Stratton et a1. al. (1984) , Solomon and Horn (1986), of (1986), and the current study used substantially different methodologies to accomplish the task of divining if all three divining the severity of officers' officers’ long-term reactions. Consequently, it is possible that if studies studies had used the same same instrument for measuring officers' officers’ post-shooting reactions during specific time frames frames and the same criteria for scoring the severity of long-term responses that 0 • differences differences between the current and previous findings findings would not have been so great. In other words, it is possible that the differences in observed severity rates between the current research words, and previous studies are not due to actual differences in officers’ officers' reactions across the three studies, but rather to differences in the methods used to arrive at the figures in question. Implications For Mental Health Training and Services Whatever the reason for the comparatively low rate of of severe responses in the current data, the fact that the officers involved in fewer than one in five of of the shootings examined data, experienced two or more negative reactions after three months had passed has at least one • 52 Solomon and Horn report that about 20% ofthe of the respondents did not indicate the amount oftliiie of lime they experienced a given reaction. In such cases, the officer was deemed to have had a severe reaction if if s’he s/he rated rated three more of of the the reactions reactions at at level level 4 or or 5. Solomon Solomon and Horn provide no information about how many many of of the the officers or more 110offered temporal information falling into into their their severe severe classification classification came from the 80% \\\Iho falling information arid and how ho", many many came from the other 20%. 20%. nor. for that matter, any information 011 from all how how many many of of the the 80% who who did did not not fall fall into into the the severe severe i f the secondary criteria were uwd to cla4sify their reactions. classification would have if used classify reactions . 8‘) This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. a • important implication for training on the aftermath of police shootings. In recent years it has become vogue in some law enforcement training circles to stress the severe negative reactions 1980; see also that befall some officers who shoot (see, (see. e.g., Adams, Adams. McTernan, and Remsberg, 1980; Everly and Mitchell, 1997 1997 for an example of the more general trend to emphasize negative psychological outcomes following critical incidents of any sort). sort). The present study suggests that this emphasis is inappropriate, and may even be counter-productive. counter-productive. It is inappropriate because stressing the severe responses that infrequently occur paints an inaccurate picture of what officers typically experience following shootings. shootings. It may be counter-productive because it may be setting officers up to have more severe reactions than they otherwise might when they do become involved in a shooting. The power of suggestion is a well-documented phenomenon (see, e.g., Rosenthal and Jacobsen, 1968), and researchers have documented the threat of iatrogenic psychological injury posed by interventions based on the assumption that those exposed to critical incidents will necessarily suffer from exposure (Gist, Lubin. and Redburn, 1999), 1999), so telling officers that they shootings might well help produce such discomfort can expect to suffer if they get involved in sho(ltings following shootings. Indeed, as reported in footnote 29, the current sample included several \vrong with them because they did not experience officers who wondered if there was something wrong the negative reactions they were told about in training, and one whose reaction mas was plainly exacerbated by his concern that he was not suffering suftcring in the fashion that he had been taughtY taught.j' In o i l post-shooting reactions should stress that most sum, the present research suggests that training on run. Indeed, given the fact that substantial portions of officers who shoot do just fine in the long run. • See ';See 53 Higgens ( I 987) for a discussion discussion of the thl'llrdical thcorctical underpinnings ofthis of this phenorneiion. H iggen~ (1987) phenomellon . \)( il(I1 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I I e • officers officers who participated in the current study experienced either zero or just a single negative reaction during the first day and week following (38% and 52%, respectively), training following a shooting (38% should similarly stress that many officers have mild short-term reactions. The findings findings that few officers suffer notable long-term consequences consequences from from involvement in shootings shootings similarly suggest that the law enforcement community should re-think some of the post-shooting mental health protocols currently in place around the nation (e.g., mandatory critical incident stress debriefings). Just as training based on the assumption that officers will experience experience problems in the wake of shootings shootings may set the stage for post-shooting problems, so to can post-shooting procedures based on the assumption that officers need mental health intervention in order to avoid expected problems. Gilbert and Silvera 1996 1996 work on that they call "overhelping" “overhelping” indicates that attempts attempts of help people who would have done just fine can have 0 • f-efficacy and thus set the stage for the unintended consequence consequence of harming their sense sense of sel self-efficacy maladjustment by weakening a faculty that is critical for sound adjustment (see page 15 15 of Gist maladjustment 1999 for a succinct discussion of this pnint). point). Thus should those charged with caring Lubin. 1999 and Lubin, shootings be aware of the potential for iatrogenic injury and take steps for officers in the wake of shootings to ensure that the helping hand they wish to offer does not instead harm. On the other hand, the evidence that officers often do just fine in the short term and only experience notable long-term problenis infrequently experience problems should not be taken as evidence that police shootings are no big deal. deal. In the first first place, the rcqearch shootings do shootings n::-.;earch clearly indicates that most shootings lead to notable disruption immediately afterward. In the second place, the research also clearly • shootings can and do lead to substantial substaritial long-term tumult for some officers. When indicates that shootings 91 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e officers suffer from from combinations of phenomena such as as sleep sleep disruption, disruption, fatigue, fatigue, nightmares, and depression more than three months after a shooting, shooting, the incident has plainly taken a substantial toll. toll. While such reactions are mercifully mercifdly infrequent, they are by no means rare and thus constitute a prominent part of the picture of what happens to police officers who shoot. shoot. Consequently, the fact that some officers suffer substantial long-term difficulties following shootings shootings should be presented in police training, but in the context of the larger picture that most officers, following following notable short-term disruption, do fine fine in the long-run. This understanding should also be incorporated into the approach that agencies take to post-shooting mental health procedures. procedures. Another question pertaining to officers' officers’ long-term adjustment that was raised by the current data is why participation in mandatory mental health counseling did not reduce the degree • of negative negati1.e reactions officers experienced after the first week following their shootings. The directed interviews shed some light on this question. Many of the officers who attended mandatory mandaton. counseling reported that they did not view the sessions as a positive experience. experience. Most of the officers who \vho held this opinion viewed the sessions as something their department required only because it was interested in "covering “covering its ass," ass,” not because it cared about the officer's officer’s wellbeing. Because they viewed the counseling sessions as a departmental departmental CYA exercise, these officers simply sought to get the through the sessions, offering as little information as possible to the MHP \tit11 Vvi th whom they met. Several of of the officers oflicers who took this approach to required counseling scssions sessions reported to the interviemer interviewer that they flat-out lied to the MHP because they did not wibh wish to divulge their thoughts, feelings, and experiences to a stranger who had ties with • their depai-tnient. department. In a related vein, other ot‘licers officers indicated that they were less than forthright 92 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. a • during their counseling sessions because they felt that the MHP they were sent to visit was incompetent. One such officer (who worked for a big city police agency) became quite animated incompetent. during the directed interview as he described his meeting with the doctor his department sent him to. When the doctor invited him into his office to officer noticed that the glasses he wore were sliding of his head because the temple on one side was missing and that the office was a mess, with books and papers piled all over the place and furniture that was in tatters. The officer assumed that the doctor was either moving into or out of the office, so he asked him which it was. When the doctor replied that he had been practicing there for quite sometime sonietime and asked the officer officer why he inquired about a move, the officer decided that it was unlikely that a doctor who could not keep his glasses stable on his head and who was unaware that his office was in • e shambles had anything to offer an officer who'd who’d just been in a shooting. It is thus possible that the null finding regarding the efficacy of mandatory post-shooting meetings with MHPs is a consequence of the context in which the counseling sessions took place. When officers do not feel comfortable, they are not likely to divulge pertinent information shootings and what they experienced afterwards. afterwards. In turn, when officers are not about their shootiiigs forthcoming during counseling sessions, it is not surprising that the sessions do not benefit 54 them. them.54 fiere not associated Wharcver the reason for the finding that mandatory MHP sessions were Whatcver long-term ther~ is substantial room m reactions, it is clear from the directed interviews is that there with long-tei 5-1 54 • \Ii\)iild also be noted here that several officers offered words of praise for the VHP’s with whom they It sh\>Llld the~HP's o f t i c c i who was contemplating contemplating suicide suicide (see (.;,e page 58), 58), for example, gave IllS his coun,e1or coiinwlor high marks for met. The offilll t h c ,ource mirce and nature of ofthe recognizing thL' the problem hhL'c was experiencing and for helping hiin him IO to resolve it. 93 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I e • for for improvement in the the delivery delivery of mental mental health services services to to officers who who become involved in shootings. shootings. The major point in this connection is is that agencies agencies must develop protocols that instill confidence among among officers officers where where post-shooting mental mental health counseling is is concerned. concerned. It should should be obvious obvious that unless officers believe that counselors counselors they meet with are are competent, competent, have the officers' officers’ best interest in mind, mind, and and are independent from the police department, department. that those officers who do suffer in the wake of shootings shootings will be quite unlikely to avail themselves of the need. mental health assistance they need. Implications for Immediate Aftermath of Shootings Shootinm Other points raised in the directed interviews interviews are relevant to how police agencies manage the aftermath of officer-involved shootings; these pertain to the immediate post-shooting procedures. The vast majority ofthe of the officers reported that they had been treated well by their • peers and supervisors at the scene of the incident, that the detectives who investigated the shooting treated them fairly,5; fairly,” and that other members of their agency likewise acted in ways that made them feel comfortable. Officers appreciated it when peers and supervisors followed what the officer felt were appropriate post-shooting procedures (e.g., protecting the shooting scene, honoring officers' officers’ requests to call loved ones), expressed concern for them, offered words of encouragement, and let the officer set the tone of of interactions; they appreciated it when the detectives explained what they were doing and conducted what the officers felt were thorough investigations; and they appreciated it when others in the agency did their jobs in a professional fashion and inquired about the officers well-being in a non-intrusive manner. • Fi Officers responded affirmatively to the statement, “"[I was treated like a suspect during the investigation of of the incident” inciLlenC' in just five cases. -55- 94 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I • a While the directed interviews interviews identified several positive points about how agencies handled the immediate aftermath of shootings, they also disclosed numerous missteps that raised officers' ire. Many of the complaints officers reported focused on how the agency managed matters at the scene of shootings. Some officers reported, for example, that despite their wishes to return to the police station for some peace and quiet, they were required to stay at the scene of the shooting for quite some time as the press arrived and crowds gathered. Others, such as the officer who witnessed a shouting-match between his attorney and the homicide supervisor (described on page 51), felt that the level l e ~ofl professionalism displayed by detectives fell short of 56 that befitting a major law enforcement investigation. In a related vein, some officers (such as in~estigation.~~ the SWAT team member mentioned on page 76 who was peeved with the detective who accused him of dishonesty about how man) many rounds he carried) complained that the detectives who • investigated their shootings were not sufficiently competent. Finally, other officers were not pleased with how the agency notified their loved ones of the shooting. shooting. Perhaps the most extreme mishandling of a family notification occurred when the involved officer specifically requested shooting. Instead lnstead of following following the officer's wishes, the that his wife not be told of the shooting. ofticer's house to pick up his wife and bring her to the department sent a squad car to the officer's scene. When the squad car delivered the wife to the scene, she was not allowed to visit shooting scene. some distance away. away. After stewing in her juices for a while with her husband, but had to wait S(lll1e about how she was being treated, she demanded a ride back home. home. When the officer found out what had happened, he was quite upset. upser 56 • soinc hours after a shooting and Another officer reported bring bt'ing d'I bit bl! upset when his captain called him some ranted on o n for several 4everal minutes about how tht' thz pt")pfe pcople managing the investigation were not nnt doing their jobs right. The officei becamc negati\e nWl1ner. manner. officel becam<: quite worried that the problciii\ probklll' afoot would affect him in a negative 56 95 95 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • a None of the the miscues miscues that the the officers officers complained complained of during during the directed interviews interviews appeared appeared to to be motivated by maliciousness,57 m a l i c i ~ u s n e s sbut , ~ ~rather seemed to to stem stem from from a lack of attention by the agencies that employed the the officers officers to developing sound post-shooting procedures. procedures. Agencies can avoid errors errors of the sorts sorts described above by training their personnel about what officers can experience in the immediate aftermath of shootings. shootings. If supervisors, supervisors, detectives, detectives, and and officers are taught that there is is a strong possibility that officers officers who shoot will be other personnel are particularly sensitive to the actions actions of others others in the wake of shootings, shootings, they will be less less likely to officers, unnecessarily accuse officers of front of officers, engage in un-professional behavior in front misfeasance, misfeasance, ignore officers' legitimate requests, or take other actions that might perturb officers. officers. SWAT officer, the detective, detective, and the 28 28 rounds in the 3D-round 30-round clip clip Finally, the story of the SWAT suggests that agencies should ensure that detectives assigned to investigate officer-involved • shootings are familiar with the nuances of the equipment that officers carry and train the shootings detectives not to jump to conclusions conclusions detectives for the investigation of officer-involved shootings shootings comes Another set of implications for from the information about officers' reactions during during shootings. Because officers so so often from so trequently frequently have imperfect recall about specific aspects experience perceptual distortions and so shootings (such as the number of rounds they fired), investigators must be aware that officers of shootings may not always be able to provide accurate information about what transpired. One implication siniply take officers' accounts of what occurred during of this is that investigators should not simply 57 57 • a coinylain that tli<it one of the homicide detectives who interviewed him told him a One officer did, however, complain bald face lit: lic about how long she had worked workcd in in the homicide unit. The officer helicved believed that the detective was using aboui the circumstances of the shooting that tliilt she felt the officer was the lie as a ploy to obtain information abOlll thar lit' froin tht' thc detectives. cletectives. withholding from her. The officer reported that he was hiding nothing from 96 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. ( 0 • their shooting as as infallible. infallible. Rather, Rather, they should take officers' accounts accounts as as a point of departure departure for for the rest of the inquiry and work back and forth forth between them and othe~ other evidence evidence (e.g., bullet trajectories and the location of shell casings) to develop the most accurate accurate possible picture of what occurred. A second implication is the flip flip of the first; first; investigators investigators should not immediately conclude that officers are being dishonest if they state that they can not recall some aspect of the event or report some information that is not consistent with other evidence. evidence. Investigators should realize that officers truly may not be able able to recall things or may have sincere beliefs that the inaccurate information they provided is correct. With such understanding in hand, investigators who are faced with problematic statements from fill in the holes or from officers can then seek to fill • 0 reconcile conflicting evidence through the investigative process. A final implication comes from the most extreme form of inaccurate recall reported by study officers: officers: having no recollection of firing one's gun. That officers can shoot and not know it suggests that investigators should check the weapons of all officers who were immediately present when the shooting occurred. occurred. Like everything else the investigators do, the reason for doing so should be explained to all officers whose guns are inspected so that the officers who fire do not feel feel as if they are being accused of lying about their actions reported that they did not fire during the shooting. By checking the weapons of all officers who could reasonably have fired and explaining why they are doing so, detectives can conduct thorough investigations without putting officers off. In sum, being cognizant of what officers may experience during shootings can help detectives conduct the thorough investigations that are necessary when the police shoot • citizens. t kens ci 97 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. i I • Future Research Needs of As the preceding list of implications indicates, the current study has yielded a good bit of useful knowledge about officers' responses to involvement in shootings. One area where the study is of limited utility, however, is on the crucial question of why officers respond as they do. Although the study did develop substantial information on the correlates of the reactions that officers experience during and after shootings, methodological limitations circumscribed its capacity to yield firm conclusions about the determinants of responses. One type of limitation lies in the measurement realm, where consideration of the validity and precision of some indicators findings might not adequately represent the true relationships in raises the possibility that some findings question. Take, for example, example, the finding that fear for self and adrenal adrenalin during shootings shootings question. in rushes during • distortions. The literature on the role that are only weakly associated with perceptual distortions. psychophysiological processes play in human perception during high stress situations suggests stronger. It is possible that the weak observed that the relationships should be substantially stronger. officers' ability to accurately rate their associations are a result (at least in part) of limitations in officers" inentaliemotional and physiological states during shootings; shootings; after all, all, we have clear-cut evidence mental/emotional that other aspects of officers' perceptions are often less than pertkct. pertect. Moreover, even if selftl:ar and the presence of elevated ratings can produce valid information regarding feelings of tkar drenalin levels, levels. the simple yes/no )es/no indicator used in the current study could be masking the adrenalin \tronger associations that more niore precise self-rating scales (e.g.. (e.g . of 01 the Likert variety) would stronger disclose. disclose. • 11i th the present research where cmlsality ciiiisal it?; is concerned is that none of Another limitation \\ith 98 98 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. 0 • relationships between officers' officers’ reactions and the various the models estimated to examine the relationships . potential determinants of them accounted for the effects of other factors. factors. ifhis This limitation is especially salient regarding what officers officers experienced in the wake of their shootings, shootings, where analysis disclosed that most of the numerous significant correlates of officers' officers’ reactions bore only mild associations. Given the weakness of these associations, the introduction of even modest controls could render the associations associations non significant. In short, short, because it is quite possible that officers’ post-shooting reactions the observed associations between the significant predictors and officers' are spurious, the findings regarding these associations should be viewed with substantial caution. Multivariate methods were not used to address the spuriousness issues in the current few cases per predictor. With more than 30 research for the simple reason that there were too few variables considered, the 113 1 13 shootings examined simply do not provide a sufficient number of of @ • officers’ reactions to cases to yield stable multivariate parameter estimates. Future research on officers' shootings can address this limitation by 1) using data reduction techniques to decrease the from larger number of predictors without losing crucial information and 2) collecting data from samples. By estimating the multivariate multivariate models that these steps would permit, future future research samples. increase the scope of knowledge regarding the determinants of officers' officers‘ postcould substantially increase develop better measures of factors factors such as the shooting adjustment. Future research should also develop fear and adrenalin rushes that officers can experience while involved in shootings in order to as they do do during such events. events. To conclude, conclude, enhance our comprehension of why officers react as study yielded a substantial substantial amount of information about police responses to while the current study further officer-involved shootings. it is clear that additional research on the topic is needed to further • clarify our understanding of this important matter. 1 99 99 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. II • Ii • • ] 00 i00 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. A • REFERENCES Adams, Ronald J., Thomas M. McTernan, and Charles Remsberg Evanston, IL: Calibre Calibre Press. 1980 1980 Street Survival: Tactics for Armed Encounters. Evanston, Artwohl, Artwohl, Alexis and Loren W. Christensen. 1997 1997 Deadly Force Encounters: What Cops Need to Know to Mentally and Physically Prepare for and Survive and Gunfight. Boulder, CO: Paladin Press. Bettinger, Keith J. J. 1983 November/December: 1983 Dreams related to post-shooting trauma. Police Marksman. NovemberDecember: 33-36. Burris, William. William. 1985 1985 Surviving Surviving the aftermath: 'Post-shooting 'Post-shooting trauma' revisited. Police Marksman May/June:31-33. May/June:3 1-33. • e Campbell, Campbell, John Henry. 1992 Comparative Analysis of the Effects of Post-Shooting Post-Shooting Trauma on the Special 1992 A Comparative of Investigation. Unpublished Ph.D. Ph.D. Dissertation. Dissertation. Agents of the Federal Bureau ofInvestigation. Department of Educational Administration, Michigan State University. Everly, George S. S. and Jeffery T. T. Mitchell. 1997 1997 Critical Incident Stress Stress Management (CISM): (CISM): A New Era and Standard Standard of Cart: Care in in Crisis Intervention. Intervention. Ellicott City, MD: Chevron. Chevron. Geller, William A. S. Scott. A. and Richard S. Know. Washington. Washington, DC: IIC: Police Executive Research Forum. t.'orum. 1992 Deadly Force: What We Know. Gersons, Berthold P.R. P,R. 1989 Patterns of PTSD among police officers following folloLcing shooting shooting incidents: A two 1989 2. 247347dimensional model and treatment implications. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2: 257. H. Silvera. T. and David 1)avid H. Gilbert, Daniel T. 1996 1996 Overhelping. 3 15-324. Overhelping. Journal .lournal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70: 315-324. • A. Resick, Resick. Susan Marhoefer-[ Marhoefer-I horak, h orak, Catherine Catherine K. Hutter. Girelli, Steven A., Patricia A. Cirelli, Subjectike distress and violence during rape: Their .I heir long-term effects on long-terlll long-tcrm 1986 Subjective fear. Violetice fear. Violence and Victims, 1:35-46. 101 101 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e Gist, Richard and Bernard Lubin. 1999 1999 Responses to Disaster: Disaster: Psychological, Community, and Ecological Approaches. Ann Arbor, MI. Braun-Brumfield. Braun-Brumfield. Richard, Bernard Lubin, and Bradley G. Redburn. Redburn. Gist, Richard, 1999 1999 Psychological, Community, and Ecological Perspective on Disaster Response. In Gist, Richard and Bernard Lubin (eds.) Responses to Disaster: Psychological, Community, and Ecological Approaches. Ann Arbor, MI. Braun-Brumfield. Community, Griffin, Michael G., Patricia A. Resick, and Mindy B. Mechanic. 1997 dissociation: Psychophysiological indicators. indicators. 1997 Objective assessment of peritraumatic dissociation: American Journal of Psychiatry, 154:1081-1088. 154: 1081-1088. Grossman, David and Bruce Siddle 1999 R. Kutz (ed.), Encyclopedia of Violence, 1999 Psychological Effects of Combat. In Lester R. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Peace, and Conflict, Volume 3. Orlando, Higgens, E. Troy. 1987 1987 Self-discrepancy: A theory relating to self and affect. Psychological Review, 94: 3193 19340. 340. • 0 Hill, Wayne R. 1984 1984 Police and post-killing trauma. Police Product News. September:57-69. September57-69, H. Range, Deirde Anglin, John Yarborough, Yarborough. Kimberly Hardaway, Marie Russell, Jared Hutson, H. Strote, Strote, Michael Canter, and Bennett Blum. 1998 1998 Suicide by cop. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 32:665-669. S. Weiss, William E. E. Schlenger, John A. Fairbank, B, ByKathleen Marmar, Charles R., Daniel S. Jordan, Richard A. Kulka, and Richard L. Hough. 1994 Peritraumatic Dissociation and Posttraumatic Stress in Male Vietnam Theater 1994 15 1 : 902-907 Veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151: Nielson, Eric. 1981 Salt Lake City Police Department Deadly Force Policy Shooting and Post Shooting 1981 Reactions. Mimiographed. Mimiographed. Rosenthal, Robert and Lenore Jacobson. 1968 Pygmalion In the Classroom. New York: 1968 York: Holt Holt. Rinehart, and Winston. Winston. • Shaw, James H. 198 1 Post-shooting trauma. trauma. Washington Law Enforcement Journal. Journal. February:22-’4. 1981 February:22-~4 . 102 102 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e Solomon, Roger M. M. and James H. Horn. Horn. Solomon, 1986 Post-shooting Post-shooting traumatic reactions: A pilot study. study. In James James T. T. Reese and Harvey A. A. 1986 for Law Enforcement Officers. Washington, Goldstein (eds.) Psychological Services for DC: U.S. Government Government Printing Office. Snibbe. Stratton, John G., David Parker and John R. Snibbe. 1984 1984 Post-traumatic Post-traumatic stress: stress: Study of police officers involved in shootings. Psychological Reports Reports 55:127-131. 55: 127-13 1. Weiss, Weiss, Daniel S., S., Charles Charles R Marmar, Marmar, Thomas J. J. Metzler, Metzler. and Heidi M. M. Ronfeldt 1995 1995 Predicting Symptomatic Symptomatic Distress in Emergency Emergency Services Personnel. Personnel. Journal Journal of Consulting Psychology, 66: 66: 361-368. Consulting • • 103 103 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. / I • TABLE 1. Percent of Officers Experiencing Specific Thoughts and Feelings at any Point During Shooting Incidents Across Different Studies a Thought/Feeling Thought/Feeling Current Study Nielson 37% 37% 137% N/M NIM NIM I N/M " Fear for Self 41% 20% 43%. 43% N/M Fear for Others 60% 40% 29% N/M Need to Survive Survive 30% 30% 22% N/M NIM N/M Adrenalin Rush 55% 55% 46% N/M NIM N/M Intrusive Thoughts 14% 14% N/M NIM N/M NfM 36% 36% Other a N/M = = Not Measured 33% N/M NIM N/M NIM N/M NIM ~ ~ TABLE 2. Officers' Thoughts and Feelings at Two Different Times During 113 113 Shooting Incidents Prior to Firing Disbelief Disbelief Fear for Self Fear for Others Need to Survive Survive Upon Firing Percent Thought,/Feeling Thought/Feeling I I I I 32% 35% 54% 27% Percent 1 I I I 34% 34O/o 30% 49'10 49% 23% 23O'O ~~ • Artwohland Artwohl and Christensen 42% 142% Disbelief I Disbelief Disbelief • Campbell Adrenalin Rush 44% 46% 46°10 Intrusive Thoughts 10% 10% 9"/o 9% Other 30% 30% 104 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. ~~~ 1 I • __ Experienced Specific Perceptual TABLE 3. Percent of Shootings in Which Officers Experienced Distortions at any Point Across Different Studies a Current Study Distortion Campbell Solomon Solomon and Horn Neilsen ArtwohI and Artwohland Christensen Tunnel Vison 51% 51% 44% 37% 43% 82% Visual Detail 56% 5 6Yo N/M NIM 18% 18% NIM N/M 65% Diminished Sound 82% 42% 51% 27% 88% Intensified Sound 20% N/M NIM 18% 18% N/M NIM 17% 17% Slow motion 56% 34% 67% 64% 63% Fast Motion 23% NIM N/M 15% 15% N/M NIM 17% 17% Other Distortion 13% NIM N/M NIM N/M NIM N/M UNK aN/M =Not Measured. UNK = Unknown, which indicates that the response was measured in some fashion, but that the nature of the item lIsed does not allow for direct translation into the specific response in the current study. • TABLE 4. Perceptual Distortions at Two Different Times Times During 113 Shooting Incidents Prior to Firing Percent Distortion Tunnel Vision 1 I Both Visual Distortiuns L)istortiuns I Auditory Blunting I Auditory Acuity Acuit! I Both Aural Distortiuns Distorti~~ns 1 Heightened Visual Detail Upon Firing Percent I 31% I I I 1 I 37% 10% 10% 42% 10% 10% 0% ~ ~ 35% 35% 11% 11% 70% 5% 5% 9% ~ ~~ Slow Motion 43% 40% Fast Motion 12% 12% 17% 17% 0% 2% Both Time Distllrtiol1s Distortions • 27% 105 lOS This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • TABLE 4. Perceptual Distortions at Two Different Times During 113 Shooting Incidents Prior to Firing Other Other I Upon Firing I 6% 6% 9% 9% ~ ~ 5. Distributions Distributions of Three Three Distortion Distortion Scale Scale Scores Scores for for 113 113 Police Police Shootings Shootings Table 5. Prior to Firing - • 1 Upon Firing Score Percent ofCases of Cases of Cases Percent ofCases 0 12% 12% 6% 6% 5% 5 yo 1 18% 18% 18% 18% 6% 2 33% 19% 19% 6% 3 31% 43% 11% 11% 4 5% 5 yo 11% 11% 16% 16% 5 1% 1Yo 4% 17% 6 0 0 29% 7 0 0 6% 6Yo 8 0 0 qf C'ases Percent o.fCases 3% 3 YO ~~ • Overall Overall ~~ 9 0 0 0 10 0 0 1% 1Yo IO6 106 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • I II Table 6. 6. Zero-Order Correlations Correlations Between 12 Specific Perceptual Distortions During 113 Shooting Incidents xl • .~ x2 x3 s3 x5 x4 x6 x7 x8 x9 x10 xlO xxII ll Tunnel Vision Prior _--- Visual Detail Prior -.38 ---- Loud Sound Sound Prior -.21 -.21 .23 ---- Reduced Sound Prior .21 .21 .03 .03 -.28 -.28 Slow Motion Prior .22 .14 .I4 -.) - . I 1) .28 .28 ---- Fast Fast Motion Prior .02 .02 -.03 -.03 .24 -.05 -.33 -.33 --_- Tunnel Tunnel Vision At SO .50 -.I9 -.19 -.0 -.OlI .os .08 -.02 .09 .09 ---- Visual Visual Detail Detail At -.IO -.10 .6I .61 .06 .IO .10 .IO .10 -.02 -.27 ---- Loud Loud Sound Sound At .10 .10 -.oo -.00 .I4 .14 .09 .03 .03 .25 -.04 .06 .06 ---- Reduced Reduced Sound Sound At At .09 .09 .07 -.04 -.04 .23 .23 .03 .03 -.06 -.06 .29 .IO .10 -.I7 -.17 Slow Motion At Slow -.o I1 -.0 .IS .18 -.04 .24 .46 .01 .01 .23 .IO .10 -.07 .32 ---- Fast Fast Motion At . I I1 .1 .05 .05 .30 .30 -.os -.08 -.05 .44 -.05 .02 .28 -.I8 -.18 -.31 x12 xl2 ---- ---- ---- 107 107 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. A • Table 7: Multidimensional Scaling Coordinates for Distortion Co-occurrence Dimension Dimension 1I Distortion Distortion .10 .10 Prior Prior Tunnel Tunnel Vision Prior Prior Visual Detail I Dimension 2 , 1.44 1.44 I -.37 -27 ~ • • ~ -1.61 -1.61 ~~ Prior Prior Auditory Auditory Amplification 1.34 1.34 -.35 Prior Prior Auditory Auditory Attenuation Attenuation -.83 -.83 .41 .41 Prior Slow Slow Motion Motion -.89 -.89 .12 Motion Prior Fast Motion 1.37 -.05 Vision At Tunnel Vision .07 .07 1.42 Visual Detail At -.35 -35 -1.41 Amplification At Auditory Amplification 1.26 1.26 -.16 Attenuation At Auditory Attenuation -2.30 .05 Slow Motion Motion At Slow -.79 .28 Fast Motion Motion At 1.38 1.38 -.14 108 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e (i • • 109 109 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. Table 8. Number of Shots Fired by Subject Officers and Number of Shots They Thought They Had Fired in 113 Shootings N N ofShots of Shots Officers Officers kt/l(fl~l' Firer! N of of Cases Cases in itz Which Wlticli Officers Officers Firer! GiI'eIl Number of Shots N ofCases uf Cuses in Which Which Officers' Officers ' Recall \1U1 Correct N oofCases f Cases in Which Officers Which 0fficei.s RcmllC!d (/ Range That Included Actual N ojShots ofCases N of Cases in Which Officers Officers Which Thought They Fired Fewer Rounds oJCases in NN qf Cuses in qfficers Which Officers had no no Clue how how had Many Rounds Rounds Mtrnv The?, They Fired Fired N ooJCases f Cases in Which Officers q[ficers Which Thought They Fired More Fired Rounds Roirnds 1 33 32 0 1 0 I 0 2 16 14 0 1 0 I 11 3 14 12 0 1 1 0 4 18 11 0 4 11 2 5 7 2 1 4 0 6 8 3 1 2 1 7 3 1 0 2 0 8 3 0 0 2 9 3 1 1 1 13 1 0 0 0 14 1 0 0 1 15 1 0 0 0 0 11 16 1 0 0 1 0 0 18 1 0 0 1 0 0 28 2 0 0 0 0 2 41 1 0 0 0 0 11 Total Cases 113 76 3 21 4 I 0 11 I - 110 110 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. 0 I : 1 0 0 0 0 1 0 I I 0 9 • Table 9. 9. Thoughts/Feelings ThoughtdFeelings Experienced Experienced at at Any Point Point Following Shooting Shooting Table Percent Thought/Fee1inq Thought/Feeling • 1044 Cases Cases Fully Fully Measured All 113 Cases Percent Elation 129% 29% 31% Sadness Sadness 26% 26% Numbness Numbness 20% 20% 21% 21% Recurrent Thoughts 83% 83% 84% 84% Anxiety 40% 43% Guilt 12% 12% 13% 13% Nightmares 18% 18% 19% 19% Fear for Safety 18% 18% 18% 18% Fear of Legal/ Administrative Administrative Fear Problems 34% 34% 3 7% 37% Any Other Thought or Any Feeling Feeling 42% 41% ~~ Experienced at Any Point Following Shooting Table 10. Physical Responses Experienced All 113 113 Cases Phvsical Physical Response • I 104 Cases Fully Measured 104 ~ Percent Percent Nausea 4% 4% Appetite Loss 17% 17% Headache 7% 7% Fatigue 46% 4-'0/ _L ° Crying 124% 24% 24~/o Trouble Trouhle Sleeping 148% 48% 50% I19% 9% I19C\o 99; Other Physical 111 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • • Thoughts/Feelings TABLE 11. Percent of Cases In Which Officers Experienced Specific ThoughtslFeelings at Any Point After Shooting Incidents, Across Different Studies a Thought/Fed Thounht/Fed ing Current Study Solomon and Horn C CampbeU Campbell b Neilsen d“ Gersons ‘‘ Gersons" N/M N/M N/M I N/M N/M I N/M 20% 120% I UNK I N/M I N/M 43% 143% N/M I N/M Recurrent thoughts 83% 23% 44% 44% 58% 76% Anxiety 40% 40% 25% UNK 33% 33% I N/M 37% 137% N/M 19% 119% 134% 34% N/M N/M I UNK UNK UNK N/M N/M Elation 29% Sadness 26% 126% Numbness Guilt 12% 112% Nightmares I 18% 18% I”N/M 1 UNK I I 1 Fear for Safety 18% 18% Fear of Legal!Admin LegaVAdmin . Problems 34% N/M N/M N/M N/M N/M Any Any Other Thought Thought or Feeling Feeling 42% UNE; UNK UNK UNK UNK N/M N/M N/M = Not Measured. UNK = Unknown, CJnknown, which \thich mdlcates indicates that the response was measured in some fashion. but 'N/M itein used does not allo"" allou for direct translation into the specific specific response in the current study stud) that the nature of the item Campbell’s data reflect reactions during the 011 b Campbell's tht: first week following shootings. The figures presented are based 011 the N of 167 that Campbell stated he used to percentage responses, although the raw frequencies and percentages he reports do not always match. Campbell used a4 single item that asked agents whether they experienced “sadness/crying/depression.” Sixteen percent (16%) ( 16%) of the agents agents responded affirmatively to this item. Given tht: the "sadness/crying/depression." io determine determine how many of these agents agents experienced sadness. sadness. nature of the item, however, it is not possible 10 Horn’s data apparently rt:flect reflect officers' officers’ responses at any point following shootings. The figures r Solomon and Horn's espondents who rated the various items at a level of three or higher highei on reported in the table reflect the percentage of Irespondents \vas designed dezigned to tap the degree to which the various varioiis responses "disrupted" “disrupted” the a Likert scale with a range of 1-5 that was officer’s life. officer's LI shootings. d Nielsen’s Nielsen's data reflect reactions during the tirst week following shootings. Gersons’s data apparently reflect officers' officers‘ tesponses lie , Gersons's responses at any point in time following shootings. The guilt item hL' wed asked officers whether they experienced exper ienced "Guilt Guilt about surviving:' surviving.“ lIsed • 112 112 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • Responses Table 12. Percent of Cases in Which Officers Experienced Particular Physical Responses ~tudies a at Any Point After Shooting Incidents, Across Different Studies Physical Phvsical Response ResDonse Current Study • ’ I Gersons Gersons ~~ Nielsen d C Nausea 4% 1% 1% N/M N M 92% N/M N M Appetite Loss 17% N/M NJM N/M N/M N/M NIM Headache 7% 5% 5% N/M 25% 125% N/M Fatigue 46% 146% 24% 24% N/M 14% 114% N/M Crying 24% UNK N/M N/M 1M Trouble Sleeping 48% 48% 32% 46% 27% 43% I • I I Solomon I and Horn I Campbell h C’umpbell I -1 I <' “ I LINK UNK NIM UNK N/M 19% UNK Other Physical m nn,, whIch which indicates in some fashIOn. fashion. but aN/M mdlcates that the response was measured III ’N/M = = Not Measured. UNK = = Unknown, Unk OM that the nature of the item used does not n : allow for direct translation into the specific response in the current study. presented are based on b Campbell's Campbell’s data reflect reactions during d ing the first week following shootings. The figures presented +h- ’hl - K 117 +I.-+ P--*hall - t O t P A hP PA rncnnncnc Q l t h n n a m h thn ir u- i vx ,v frequencies f b n i * c - n p ; n c I n A nnrpnntsonc h e the percentages iuai Campbell b a l l l p G 1 1 stated J L c I t L u he Llsed to percentage responses, although the raw iiryubiirir~and cuiu y v i v v x s c u s - ~ he LIIC N I Y of V I 167 1 w 1 that used a single item that asked agents whether they experienced reports do not always match. match. Campbell med "sadness/crying/depression." “sadness/crying/depression.” Sixteen percent (16%) ( 16%) of the agents agents responded affirmatively to this item. Given the nature of the item, however, it is not possible to determine how many of these agents cried. In a related vein. Campbell did include an "other" “other” physical phLsical response category in his study. Because the specific response categories he used are different from from those in the current study, however, his "other" “other” category is not directly comparable to the one used in the current study. C Solomon and Horn's Horn’s data apparently apparentlj reflect officers' officers’ responses responses at any point following shootings. The figure figure reported for "trouble “trouble sleeping" sleeping” is the percentage of respondents who rated "sleep “sleep disturbances" disturbances” at a level of three or higher on a Likert scale with a range of 1-:; that was designed to tap the degree I -i degree to which sleep sleep disturbances "disrupted" “disrupted” the officer's officer’s life. life. d Nielsen's Nielsen’s data reflect reactions reactions during the [he first first week following shootings. shootings. The figures for the response category "Nausea" ”Nausea” are based on an item that asked officers whether they had experienced "NausealUpset “NauseaAJpset Stomach." Stomach.” Nielsen N ielsen reported two sets of figures figures on post-shooting fatigue; fatigue; one that he identifies as as a "physical “physical symptom" symptom” and one that he identifies as an "emotional Tlie 14% figure in the table is what he reported under the physical symptom “emotional symptom'-· symptom .* The heading (he reported that officers officers experienced emotional fatigue in 25% of the cases). Nielsen did include an "other" i n his.;tudy. his .tudj. Because the specific response categories he used are different from “other” physical response category in those in the current study, however. his "lither" study, --other“category is is not directly comparable comparable to the one used in the current curl ent study. \tiid) < Gersons's (iersons’s data apparently apparently reflect otlilL'rs' otticcrs’ responses at any point in time following shootings. The 43% reported for tor "trouble “trouble sleeping" sleeping” is the percentage percent‘ige 01 D I respondents who reported experiencing experiencing "sleep “sleep disturbances." disturbances.” LLL I t n nnrPPnt-nn UJLU I W ~ L B ~ L ~ L ~ L ~ UJ ~~ ~W ~ ~ LLIIIIWUSII J U J , ]113 13 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. L Z L ~ / I • Table 13. 13. Percent Percent of Cases in in Which Officers Officers Experienced Experienced Particular Particular Thoughts Thoughts or or Table Feelings During Four Post-Shooting Post-Shooting Time Periods Periods Feelings Thouaht/Feeling ThoughtlFeeling Week First Week (N=I13) (N=l13) Within Three Months (N= 111) After Three Months (N=105) Elation 126% 26% 19% 119% 11% 5% Sadness Sadness 18% 118% 17% 117% 5% 5% 5% 5 yo Numbness 118% 18% 7% 17% 4% 4% 3% 3% I 82% 82% 74% 174% 52% 52% 3 7% 37% 37% 37% 28% 28% 13% 13% 110% 10% Recurrent thoughts thoughts Anxiety • Hours First 24 Hours (N=l12) (N=112) Guilt 110% 10% 15% 5% 6% 6% 2% 2% Nightmares Nightmares 113% 13% 13% 113% 10% 10% 6% 6% for Safety Fear for 1 9% 9% 10% 110% 9% 9% 8% 8% Fear of Legal Fear 4dministrative Administrative Problems Problems 31% 31% 25% 25% 19% 19% 11% 11% Any Other Any rhought or Thought Feeling Feeling 33% 33% 23Yo 23% 20% 14% 14% Table 14. 14. Percent Percent of Cases in Which Officers Officers Experienced Experienced Particular Physical Responses Responses Table During Four Post-Shooting Time Periods ~~ • Phvsical Physical ResDonse Response First 24 Hours (N=l12) (N=112) Firs! First Week Week (N=l13) (N=113) Within Three Three Within Months Months (N=111) (N=l 1 1) A$er Three Three After Months (N=105) (N=105) Nausea 4% 4% 4% 4% 0% 0% 0% 0% Appetite Loss 16% 16% 8% 8Yo 2% 1 Yo 1% I leadache Headache 6% 6% 4% 4% 1% 1 Yo 1 Yo 1% I‘atigue Fatigue 39% 39% 26% 26% 7% 5% 5% :‘rying Crying 17% 7yo 7% 2% 2 YO 2% I’rouble I'rouble <leeping Sleeping 46% 46% 36% 36% 16% 16% 11% 11% 114 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • e Responses Table 14. Percent of Cases in Which Officers Experienced Particular Physical Responses During Four Post-Shooting Time Periods Other Physical 18% 118% 11% 111 % 12% 112% 6% 16% Frequencies of Post-Shooting Response Scale Scores for Four Time Periods Table 15. Frequencies • • Scale Scule Score First 24 Hours (N=1l2) (N=l12) First Week Week (N=I13) (N=l13) 0 23 42 42 I 6464 66 166 1 20 17 17 20 120 I 1199 2 15 15 14 14 10 10 10 10 3 16 16 11 11 4 4 4 12 13 1 66 3 5 7 6 1 22 2 12 66 I 88 6 4 0 7 1 2 1 00 1 00 8 7 00 1 1 9 2 1 0 l 0o 10 10 0 0D l 0o l 0o 11 II 0 03 0 0 12 12 1 03 0 13 13 0 11 l 0o Mean Mean 2.88 2.88 2.05 Within After Three Within Three Three Afer Three Months (N=III) 05) Monrhs (N=l11) Months (N=1 (N=l05) 1 06 115 115 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. l 0o l 0o .77 ( • Figure 1. 1. Percent Percent of of 113 113 Cases Cases in in Which Which Officers Officers Figure to Firing Firing ExperiencedVisual Visual Distortions Distortions Prior Prior to Experienced Both Distortions Distortions 10% IC Both No No Distortion Distortion 22% 22% Detail 37% 37 Increased -_Increased Detail - Tunnel 31% Tunnel Vision Vision 31% • Figure 2. 2. Percent of 113 113 Cases in in Which Officers Officers Figure Distortions While Firing Firing ExperienIced Visua Experienced VisualI Distortions Both Dostortions 11 11 % Distortion 27% No Distortion Increased Detail 35% Tunnel Vision Vision 27% 27% Tunnel • L 116 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I • Figure 3. 3. Percent Percent of 113 113 Cases Cases in in Which Which Officers Officers Figure Experienced Auditory Distortions Distortions Prior to Firing Firing Experienced Increased Sound 10'1'0 No No Distortion Distortion 49% Reduced Sound 42% *• Figure 4. 4. Percent Percent of 113 113 Cases in in Which Officers Officers Figure Experienced Auditory Distortions Distortions While Firing Firing Experienced Both Distortions 8% No Distortion 17% 17% Increased Sound 5% Reduced Sound 70% • 117 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • Figure Figure 5. 5. Percent Percent of 113 1I 3 Cases in in Which Which Officers Officers Experienced Time Time Distortions Distortions Prior to Firing Firing Experienced Fast Motion Motion 12% 12% No 4 0 No Distortion 44% - Motion 43% 43% Slow Motion • Figure 6. 6. Percent Percent of 113 113 Cases in in Which Officers Officers Figure Experienced Experienced Time Distortions Distortions While Firing Firing Both Both Distortions 2% Fast Motion Motion 17% 17% No Distortion 42% Slow Motion 40% • 118 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • • Figure 7: Scaterplot of Multidimensional Scaling of Perceptual Distortions N t: o (/) c: (1) E CI Dimension 1 119 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • • • • Figure 8: 8: Percent Percent of of Cases Cases With With Given Given Post-Shooting Post-Shooting Scale Scale Scores Scores for for Four Four Tim~ 'rim?Periods Periods Figure 70 70 60 60 50 v) CIl 40 j--.,-...........""±\.,~;......,...-"-~~~~-+:-:: ~ 40 co -+--tFirst First Day Day (N=112) (N=lI2) rc 0 -._ --f First Week (N=113) (N=l13) ~ u () o + ..... C t: ~~ ~~ Prior to to Three Months Months (N=111) ( N = l I 1) al ell 2 30 ~ 30 Q) After Three Months Months (N=105) (N=lO5) ell a a.. -~ 20 20 10 0 o 0o 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Scales Scores 120 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. 10 11 11 12 1133 ~ ~ ~ I *• DEADLY FORCE FORCE QUESTIONNAIRE PARTII PART 1. shooting __/_ 1. Date of ofshooting / _/__ / mo day day yr mo 2. 2. Approximate Approximate time time of shooting shooting (military )) _ 3. 3. Your age (in years) at time of shooting shooting _ _ Ii 4. 4. Sex:(check one) one) Male _ _,,Female_ Female _ 5. 5. Race/ethnicity:(check Race/ethnicity:(check one) White _ _,,Black __,,Hispanic _'_, ’ , Asian _ _,,Other__ Other 6. 6 . Are you a military veteran? veteran? Yes__, Yes ,No__ No If"yes," If “yes.” do you have combat experience? experience? Yes__, Yes ,No__ No7. 7. Years and months as a police officer at time of shooting shooting (e.g., (e.g., 10 10 years, 2 moths)_ moths) _/_ / _ yrs mos 8. Rank at time of shooting (e.g., officer, sergeant, lieutenant): _ • 9. 9. Type Type of law enforcement enforcement agency you worked for at time of shooting: (Check one) one) Municipal __._Municipal _ _County County State Federal _ _Other Other (e.g., school district, transit, etc.) _ (Please specify) 10. Had you worked for a different law enforcement agency prior to this shooting? 10. shooting? Yes .N o No 1 1. How many incidents have you been involved in where you fired shots? 11. shots?_ _ 12. How many incidents where other officers, but not you, fired shots? shots?_ _ • 13. 13. How many incidents have you been involved in where you believe you could have legally fired sliots. shots. but you did not fire? fire?_ _ This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. 1 • 14. for: (Check one) 14. Type Type of law enforcement agency agency you currently work for: _ _Municipal MuniciDal _ _County County State State Federal _ _Other Other (e.g., (e.g., school district, district, transit, etc.) i( _ (Please specify) specify) _ _No No longer in law enforcement 15. 15. Current rank (e.g., (e.g., officer, officer, sergeant, sergeant, lieutenant): lieutenant): _ 16. 16. Activity/assignment at time of shooting: shooting: (Check one) General General Patrol Patrol Traffic Traffic Patrol Patrol • _ _ Special Special Patrol Patrol (e.g., (e.g., crime suppression, suppression, anti-gang, etc.) Detective; search warrant service Detective; Detective; arrest warrant service or other apprehension _ _ Detective; SWAT _ SWAT (Please specify operation type; e.g, hostage, warrant service, service. etc) (Please Undercover Undercover _ _ Offduty Off duty _ _ Other; Other; please specify _ 17. 17. Were you married at the time of the shooting? Yes- -,,No No - If "yes." “yes.” were you separated? separated? Yes _ _ , No_ No- _ Y If "no" “no“ were you divorced? divorced? Yes - -, No- 7 • 18. 18. Ifmarried If married at time of shooting, are you now married to the same person? Yes- -,,No No-- - This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. 2 • 19. 19. Were civilians who were not suspects (i.e., victims, bystanders, etc.) present at the scene of shooting? Yes__, Yes-, No “yes,” how many? _ _ the shooting? No__ If "yes," 20. 20. Were other law enforcement officers officers present at the time you fired? (check all that apply) _ _No, No, I was alone _ _Yes, Yes, other officers from from my agency were present _ _Yes, Yes, officers from other agencies were present 21. If you were not alone, how many other officers from your own agency were present? _ _,, how many officers from other agencies? _ _ 22. 22. If other officers from your agency were present, how many of them fired fired rounds during this incident? 23. If If officers from other agencies were present, how many of them fired rounds during this 23. • a incident? incident?24. How many suspects were present during the shooting incident?_ incident? _ (If more than three suspects, please ask the interviewer for an “Additional "Additional Suspect” Suspect" sheet to be used for the next item.) 25. This item refers to the weapons possessed by suspect(s) (check all that apply) SusDect #1 was armed with: Suspect I Suspect #2 was armed with: _ _ Blunt Object (e.g.,Bat/Club) _ _ Blunt 0 Object bject __Blunt Blunt Object _ _ Edged weapon (e.g, Knife) _ _Edged Edged Weapon __Edged Edged Weapon _ _ Handgun Handgun __ Shotgun - • Suspect #3 was armed with: I SusDect __Handgun Handgun _ _Shotgun Shotgun __Shotgun Shotgun Rifle Rifle Rifle Other Other Other (specify) Unarmed (specify) Unarmed I - This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. 1 - t(specify) specify) Unarmed • 3 26. If If any any suspect possessed possessed a firearm, did did suspect(s) fire at at you? you? Yes-, Yes_ _, No No_ _,,UnknownUnknown__ 26. If yes, how many total rounds were fired at you? you?_ _ If ,No-,Unknown 27. If If any suspect possessed possessed a firearm, did suspectis) suspect(s) fire at other officers Yes Yes_ _,No_,Unknown__ If yes, how many total rounds were fired at other officers? officers?_ _ If If any suspect possessed a firearm, did suspect(s) fire at any citizens? 28. If Yes_ _,No_ unknownYes ,No _,UnknoWll__ ? If yes, how many total rounds were fired at other citizens?citizens? If 29. Approximately how many minutes elapsed between the time you arrived at the shooting location and the time you fired your first round? (less than one minute =1)- 30. Total number of of rounds you fired during this incident _ fire at? at?_ _ 331. 1. How many suspects did you fire • gunfire?_ _ 32. How many suspects did you hit with gunfire? 33. How many suspects were struck by rounds fired by other officers?_ officers? _ 34. 34. The most serious wounds suffered by suspect susDect #1 #1 were: Fatal _ _Serious Serious (i.e., &e., required hospitalization) hospitalization) Minor 35. 35. The The most serious serious wounds suffered suffered by by suspect #2 #2 were: were: -FatalFatal __Serious Serious (i.e., (Le., required hospitalization) hospitalization) Minor • 36. 36. The The most most serious serious wounds wounds suffered suffered by by suspect suspect #3 #3 were: were: Fatal Fatal This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • 0 _ _Serious Serious (i.e., (i .e.,required hospitalization) hospitalization) 4 Minor 37. 37. Did you provide first aid to any any suspect(s)? suspect(s)? Yes__, Yes ,No_ No _.. 38. 38. Did other officers officers provide first first aid to any suspect(s)? Yes_ Yes _, No__ No-9 39. No _ If yes, were you hospitalized for for 39. Were you injured during during the shooting shooting incident? incident? Yes_ Yes _,,No_ I/ II treatment? treatment? Yes_ Yes _,,No_ No _ 40. Did anyone other than suspect(s) fatal wounds? Yes_ suspect(s) suffer any fatal Yes _,,No_ No _ If yes, who? (Check all that apply) Citizen _ apply) Police officer_ officer _,,Citizen_ 41. Did anyone other than self or suspect(s) suspect(s) suffer any non-fatal wounds? Yes- -,,No- If yes, who? (Check all that apply) apply) Police Police officer_ officer _,,Citizen_ Citizen _ 42. Had you had any prior personal contact with the suspect(s) you shot? Yes_ No_ _ Yes _,,No • e (e.g., from from prior arrest) arrest)If yes, please describe (e.g., experienced during the incident, prior to firing firinn first shot: thoughVfeeling you experienced 43. Check each thought/feeling Disbelief that the incident was happening __ Disbelief Fear for self Fear for others __ Feeling that "I "I must survive" __ Rush of strength or adrenalin Thoughts about irrelevant matters (e.g., __Thoughts (e.g., family, friends, past experiences, etc.) -OtherOther - - - (Please - - -describe) --------- 44. Check each perceptual distortion you experienced prior to firing your first shot: • @ Visual Distortion: -Tunnel Tunnel vision Heightened detail _ _Heightened Auditory Distortion: -Diminished Diminished sound Intensified sound _ _Intensified This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • Time Distortion: Slow motion Other Distortions: -Fast Fast motion _ 5 (Please describe) 45. Check each thoughtlfeeling thought/feeling you experienced upon or after firing first shot: 45. Disbelief that the incident was happening Disbelief Fear for self Fear for others (e.g., fellow officers, bystanders, etc.) _ _ Feeling that AI must survive@ survive@ _ _ Rush of of strength or adrenalin __Thoughts family, friends, past experiences, etc.) Thoughts about irrelevant matters (e.g., family, Other-~------------(Please describe) • 0 46. 46. Check each perceptual distortion you experienced upon or after firing firing your vour first shot: Visual Distortions: Auditory Distortions: Time Distortions: Tunnel vision -Tunnel __Heightened Heightened detail Diminished sound _ _Slow Slow motion Intensified Intensified sound Fast motion Other Distortions: - - - (Please - - -describe) --------describe) 47. Check all physical responses you experienced within the first first 24 hours after the shooting: shooting: Nausea __Loss Loss of appetite Headaches -Headaches __Fatigue Fatigue __Crying -Crying • 0 Trouble falling/staying falling/staying asleep asleep -__Trouble -OtherOther - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. (Please describe) • 6 48. Check all all thoughts/feelings you experienced within the first 24 hours after the shooting: Elation Sadness Sadness Numbness Recurrent thoughts about the shooting Fear for safety __Fear Fear oflegal of legal and/or administrative problems __Anxiety Anxiety Guilt • __Nightmares N i ght mare s -OtherOther _ (Please describe) 49. 49. Check all all physical responses you experienced between the second and seventh days after the shooting (i.e., within the first week, but after the first day) : shooting Nausea -Loss __Loss of appetite Headaches -Fatigue __Fatigue __Crying -Crying Trouble falling/staying falling/staying asleep __Trouble • -OtherOther - - -(Please - - -describe) ---------(Please describe) This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • 77 50. Check Check all all thoughts/feelings thoughts/feelings you you experienced experienced between between the the second second and and seventh seventh daw days after after the the 50. shooting (Le., (i.e., within within the the first first week, week, but but after after the the first first day) day) :: shooting Elation Elation Sadness Numbness _ _Recurrent Recurrent thoughts about the shooting Fear for safety Fear of oflegal legal and/or administrative problems • _ _Anxiety Anxiety Guilt Nightmares Other - - -(Please - - -describe) ---------51. 5 1. Check all all physical responses responses you experienced between the eighth day and third month following following the shooting (i.e., within the first three months, but after the first first week): week): Nausea _ _Loss Loss of appetite appetite Headaches _ _Fatigue Fatigue _ _Crying -Crying • e - __Trouble Trouble falling/staying falling/staying asleep asleep Other Other- - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. i l/ • (Please (Please describe) 8 52. day and third month following 52. Check all thoughts/feelings you experienced between the eighth dav the shooting (i.e., (i.e.. within the first first three month~, month?, but after the first week): Elation Sadness Numbness _ _Recurrent Recurrent thoughts about the shooting _ _Fear Fear for safety _ _Fear Fear of legal and/or administrative problems _ _Anxiety Anxiety • Guilt _ _Nightmares Nightmares Other - - -(Please - - -describe) ---------- 53. Check all all physical responses responses you experienced after the third month following the shooting: 53. Nausea Loss of appetite appetite _ _Loss Headaches _ _Headaches _ _Fatigue Fatigue _ _Crying Crying Trouble falling/staying falling/staying asleep _ _Trouble • Other - - -(Please ------------(Please describe) This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • 9 54. 54. Check Check all all thoughts/feelings you experienced after the third month following the shooting: Elation Sadness Sadness Numbness _ _Recurrent Recurrent thoughts about the shooting _ _Fear Fear for for safety _ _Fear Fear of legal and/or administrative administrative problems _ _Anxiety Anxiety Guilt _ _Nightmares Nightmares • Other ---------------(Please (Please describe) describe) Yes_ _,, NoNo_ _ 55. 55. Did your agency give you time off (non-punitive) after the shooting? Yes 56. Have you ever spoken with a mental health professional (e.g., psychologist, psychiatrist) 56. psychiatrist) about the shooting? shooting? (Check all that apply) _ _Yes, Yes, my agency required me to do so _ _Yes, Yes, II did so so on my own N Noo 57. Check all responses that fellow officers expressed to you about your shooting: 57. support _ _Support Curiosity _ _Curiosity • Criticism Aggravation _ _Aggravation This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • Other ----------------(Please (Please describe) describe) 10 10 58. 58. Check all all responses responses that superior officers officers expressed expressed to you about your shooting: shooting: _ _Support support _ _Curiosity Curiosity Criticism _ _Aggravation Aggravation Other _ describe) (Please describe) 59. Check all responses that family family members expressed to you about your shooting: shooting: _ _Support support _ _Curiosity Curiosity • Criticism Fear Other ---------------(Please describe) 60. Check all responses that non-law enforcement friends expressed to you about your shooting: _ _Support support _ _Curiosity Curiosity Criticism Fear Other _ (Please describe) of the shooting, have you had any contact with: The suspect? Yes Yes_ _,, No-; No__; 61. Since the date of • a His/her family? family? Yes Yes_ _, No No_ _;;His/her Hislher friends? friends? Yes Yes_ _, NoNo_ _ Hidher 62. Prior to this shootintr, shooting, had you participated in any officer safety/survival training other than This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • what you received in the basic academy you attended? Yes__, No_ _ Yes ,No- 11 11 63. 63. Do you feel that whatever training you did receive prior to this shooting prepared you adequately for this shooting? __, No__ , Nofor this shooting? Yes Yes 64. 64. Check all persons with whom you discussed this shooting in detail: _ _Spouselboy Spousehoy or girlfriend _ _Other Other family members Fellow officers _ _Supervisors -Supervisors _ _Clergy Clergy Other ----------------(Please specify) 65. 65. Who provided you with substantial support following following this shooting? (Check all that apply) e - • _ _Spouselboy Spousehoy or girlfriend __Other Other family members Fellow officers __Supervisors Supervisors _ _Clergy Clergy Other. _ - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - (Please specify) specify) 66. Who conducted the investigation into this shooting? (Check all that apply) 66. _ _My My own agency _ _Another Another law enforcement agency _ _District/County/State=s District/County/State=s Attorney 0 • U.S. Attorney/Department of Justice -_ _U.S. None was conducted This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I • 0 12 67. Yes_ _,,NoNo_ _ 67. Was Was your your weapon weapon held held as as evidence? evidence? Yes If Ayes,@ Yes_ _, No No_ _ Ayes,@were you issued another weapon? Yes-, 68. ~here you and/or your agency were named as a 68. Did this shooting shooting result in any civil litigation where defendant? Yes_ No _ Yes _,,No_ 69. Yes_ _,,NoNo_ _ 69. Did you obtain legal advice regarding this shooting? Yes 70. 70. In your estimation, estimation, the press coverage of this shooting was: Extensive -Extensive Moderate Minimal Minimal Non-existent • e 71. 71. Check Check all all categories categories of people who caused you aggravation about this shooting: Fellow officers officers _ _Superior Superior officers Politicians _ _Non-police Non-police friends friends -NewsNews media office Prosecutors office -Suspect's _ _Suspect's Attorney Suspect's friends friends and/or family family _ _Suspect's Other Other ---------------(Please specify) (Please specify) • 0 72. Please Please indicate indicate your highest level of education attained: attained: 72. at time time of the the shooting. shooting. High School_ School _,,Some Some College_ College _, BA/BS_ BA/BS _". More than BA/BS at BA/BS_ _ 3- This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. at at present present time. time. @ • High School_ BAlBS_ _ School _,,Some Some College_ College _,,BA/BS_ BA/BS _,,More than BA/BS 13 PARTII PART11 I Below is is a series series of statements statements representing what some officers who have' have’been involved in shootings have have had to to say say about about involvement in shootings. Some Some will apply to you, others will not. Please place a check check in in the the space space preceding each statement that represents your experience. A person who has has not been in a shooting incident can't can’t really understand what it is like. - -A Whatever happens in the future, future, 1I think 1I will be able to handle it. -_ _The marriage/relationship with my girllboyfriend. The incident led to problems in my marriagelrelationship _ _Thoughts Thoughts or memories about the shooting kept coming into my mind. ___II think that the whole thing made me a better person. _ _II felt for the subject who was shot. shot. felt sorry for _ _The The shooting helped me to grow/mature. grow/mature. • _ _II was treated like a suspect during the investigation of the incident. _ _My My sense sense of humor helped me to cope with the shooting. _ _The The whole incident made me reevaluate what was important in my life/my goals and values. _ _Because ifI'll Because of the shooting, shooting, 1I sometimes wonder if I’ll be able to face what the future may bring. The way it was handled afterwards was more harmful to me than the shooting itself. _ _The _ _II felt that 1was I was made a scapegoat after the incident. _ _II was helped by my religious beliefs and/or practices. _ _II can remember the shooting as if it happened yesterday. _ _II was disappointed by my spouse/boy/girlfriend’s spouse/boy/girlfriend's reaction to the incident. sometimes felt felt guilty about what happened. _ _II sometimes It’s very hard for me to find anything good about the incident and what followed. _ _It's followed . • This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. ( 14 • PREFACE EACH OF shooting: OF THE REMAINING STATEMENTS WITH THE TERM After the shooting: _ _II slept more poorly than usual. _ _II felt angry, angry, and it helped me. _ _II learned that 1I could trust people, and count on them in a crisis. 1I was more irritable at home and had a "shorter “shorter fuse." fuse.” _ _II became more more interested in/involved irdinvolved with my work. I became less interested in/involved irdinvolved with my work. - -1became _ _My My family family was bitter towards the agency 1I worked for. _ _II felt regret over injuring someone/taking someonehaking a life. _ _My My spouse was very worried/upset. worried/upset. • _ _It It was harder for me to feel things. e- friends, or ,Lisure leisure activities. irdinvolved with my hobbies, houuies, frlznds, - -1I became more interested in/involved irdinvolved with my hobbies, friends, or leisure activities. _ _II became less interested in/involved _ _II was more irritable with other people at work. irdinvolved with my family. _ _II became more interested in/involved idinvolved with my family. family. _ _II became less interested in/involved felt harassed and/or blamed by other people after the shooting. _ _II felt My future future will be better than my past. _ _My _ _II worried a lot about the investigation into the shooting. less cautious/concerned about situations that might involve firearms or dangers. _ _II became less _ _II became more cautious/concerned about situations that might involve firearms or danger. a- • My child(ren) child(ren) were very worried/upset. worriedupset. _ _My hyper-alert. _ _II became hyper-alert. This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. I( • 15 15 1 startled more easily than before. - -I startled more easily than before. _ _It helped me me to to help/listen help/listen to to others others who who had had been been involved involved in in shootings. shootings. It helped _ _It helped me me to to share share experiences experiences and and feelings feelings with with others others who who had had been been involved involved in in shootings. shootings. It helped _ _II trusted people people less than than before. before. _ _II felt the need need to to apologize to to the the suspect’s suspect's family. _ _It It helped me to get back to my normal work routine. _ _My worried/upset. My parents were very worriedhpset. _ _My My reaction to the incident was influenced by other shootings I1 had been involved in. _ _II dreamed frequently about the shooting. • _ _I had bad dreams about things that were not related to the shooting. _ _I felt that my happiest days are in the past. _ _II dreamed more after the incident, but the dreams were not frightening or unpleasant. _ _The The people who should have supported supported me were all busy "covering “covering their asses." asses.” _ _II drank drank more alcoholic beverages than before. before. _ _II had more trouble remembering things than before. before. _ _II had more trouble concentrating than did before. before. than I did 1tried I tried to to avoid avoid situations situations similar similar to to it. it. 1tried I tried to to avoid avoid situations situations that reminded reminded me me of it. it. _ _It It helped helped me me to to keep keep my my mind mind off off what what had had happened. happened. _ _Most Most people people were were insensitive insensitive to to what what II had had gone gone through. through. • _ _II sometimes was happening happening again, again, especially especially if! if I was was in in aa similar similar situation. situation. sometimes felt felt like like itit was 0_ - _II feltfelt angry, angry, and and itit upset upset me. me. This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. • 16 _ _II felt that I was the only one who really cared about me. _ _II reviewed the incident again and again, wondering if if I did the right thing. _ _II had trouble explaining what happened to my children. - -I felt more isolated from other people than I did before it. _ _II felt 'uncomfortable/insecure uncomfortable/insecure about being alive. _ _II felt worse in situations that reminded me of of the shooting. shooting. - -It helped me to be physically active. _ _II mostly wanted to be left alone, alone, even by people who were trying to help me. - -It helped me to talk with other officers who had been involved in shooting incidents. • _ _It It helped me to hear about shootings that other officers had been involved in. Thank you for your assistance in completing this questionnaire. F' FiEHTY v OFService (NCJRSJ ' CrimlilaJ Jusrice Reference 6000Criminal Justice Reference Service (NCJRS) tJ • This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. ( • 17 Ii • • This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.